Eradication of norway rats (rattus norvegicus) from hawea island, fiordland, using brodifacoum
R.H. TAYLOR and B.W. THOMAS
Ecology Division, Department of Scientific and Industrial Research, Nelson.
ERADICATION OF NORWAY RATS
(RATTUS NORVEGICUS)FROM HAWEA ISLAND, FIORDLAND, USING BRODIFACOUM
Summary: Norway rats were eradicated on bush-covered Hawea Island (9 ha) in Breaksea Sound, using the
anticoagulant rodenticide "Talon 50 WB" (brodifacoum). The work was done as a conservation measure and to
evaluate the feasibility and costs of eradicating rodents quickly from islands. The 50-100 rats present were
eradicated in about two weeks by applying a simple strategy that took full account of the characteristics of the
poison, the environment, and the behaviour of the target species. The technique used was designed to monitor
its own progress, kill every rat as quickly as possible, continually detect the presence of surviving rats, limit the
risk to non-target species, and overcome the many problems often associated with "getting the last rat".
Keywords: Norway rat;
Rattus norvegicus; Rodentia; Fiordland; Hawea Island; Breaksea Sound; New Zealand;
eradication; poisoning; brodifacoum.
IntroductionIn eradication campaigns when few of the target
rattus), and kiore
(R. exulans) from several New
species are left, the operations usually become
Zealand islands up to 22 ha in extent (Moors, 1985a;
protracted, costs become increasingly high for each
Taylor, 1984a; Towns, 1988). Following long-drawn-
animal killed, and field staff tend to lose motivation.
out campaigns against Norway rats on the Noises
Unfortunately, there is also a tendency for managers,
Islands, Moors (1985a) concluded that "the last few
or those providing finance, to stop the campaign
rats are certainly the most expensive and exacting to
before eradication is achieved, on the grounds of
destroy, but they are also obviously the most vital if
excessive costs without any certainty of success
the campaign is to succeed". He also found that the
(Merton, 1978).
greatest practical difficulty encountered was detecting
In 1976, Yaldwyn (1978) concluded a conference
the continued presence of rats at very low density
on the ecology and control of rodents in New Zealand
(Moors, 1986).
by stating that the possibility of complete
Most recent commentators on methods of rat
extermination of rodent populations from New
extermination have stressed the difficulty of getting
Zealand offshore islands was "remote, or at least a
the last rat, and the importance of using as many
very, very difficult thing indeed". By the early 1980s
methods of killing rats as possible and never relying
it was still widely held that no real breakthrough in
on one weapon alone (Moors, 1985a; Wace, 1986).
this field was in sight (Atkinson, 1986).
Moors' (1985a) plan of campaign was to use sodium
Today the scene is very different. New, potent
monofluoroacetate (1080) to try and obtain a rapid
and highly palatable "second-generation"
initial reduction in rat numbers, and then follow up
anticoagulant poisons have been developed to improve
with the anticoagulant brodifacoum "against those
control of rats and mice, and to combat the genetic
rats which had become shy of 1080, or had not been
resistance to warfarin that has developed in many
attracted to bait containing 1080".
populations of rodents throughout the world
Repeated control with poison or traps represents
(Greaves, 1985). Unlike warfarin and other earlier
a major selective pressure on Norway rat populations
anticoagulants, second-generation anticoagulants kill
(Barnett, 1975). One result is the very rapid
after a single feeding and so dispense with the need
development of genetical resistance to frequently used
for ingestion over several days. Rats will consume a
poisons (Howard, Marsh and Palmateer, 1973;
lethal dose well before they begin to experience toxic
Greaves, 1985). Another is an increase in the level of
effects (Redfern, Gill and Halder, 1976), and unlike
neophobia (sometimes called "new object reaction")
many other acute or chronic poisons, sub-lethal doses
among members of controlled populations (Shorten,
are not known to cause bait aversion in Norway rats
1954; Cowan, 1977). Neophobia is an inherited
tendency - which can be reinforced by learning -
Since the early 19805, these new poisons have
for animals to avoid any new object or changed
been used to eradicate Norway rats, ship rats
(R.
situation in the environment. Of course all new things
New Zealand Journal of Ecology l2:New Zealand Ecological Society
NEW ZEALAND JOURNAL OF ECOLOGY, VOL. 12, 1989
will eventually become familiar - so the effect isalways temporary (Barnett, 1975). Usually theavoidance of new bait stations lasts from one to tendays, but novel baits in familiar surroundings areoften accepted in one or two days (Elton and Ranson,1954). Another behavioural response is a learnedaversion to a poison, type of bait, or bait station.
Such "bait shyness" occurs in individuals that havepreviously suffered from sub-lethal poisoning, and itseffects can be long lasting (Rzoska, 1953; Chitty,1954).
With these problems in mind, our approach to
practical rat eradication from islands has been todevelop a strategy that will be closely adhered tothroughout the campaign and require little, if any,additional input to get the last rat. We aim for aplanned programme for each island that takes account
Figure 1:
Islands at the entrance of Breaksea Sound.
of the behaviour and ecology of the target species andall aspects of the local environment. This programmeshould progressively kill rats in the shortest possible
to eradicate rats from the large (170 ha) and rugged
time, continually monitor its own progress, detect the
Breaksea Island (Thomas and Taylor, 1988), where
presence of surviving rats, limit the risk to non-target
Norway rats have affected the population densities of
species, and - most importantly - maintain the
many invertebrate groups (Bremner, Butcher and
confidence of administrators and the morale of the
Patterson, 1984).
campaign personnel. We consider that for each
A concurrent study of the effects on Hawea
campaign the operators should select the single best
Island's other biota from rat eradication (Taylor,
method available and employ it with the aim of
Thomas and Taylor, 1986; Taylor and Thomas, 1986)
achieving a 100%
kill in the shortest possible time. If
is still underway and will be reported separately.
this initial plan fails, then contingency techniques canbe resorted to, but it will be easier to understand any
problems if the effectiveness of each killing method is
Hawea Island (45o 35 'S, 166 o 38 'E) is a 9 ha bush-
clad island in the entrance of Breaksea Sound,
The work reported here involved the eradication
Fiordland National Park. A small vegetated islet,
of a population of Norway rats from Hawea Island,
"The Hump", lies less than 5 m off its southern shore
Fiordland, using the anticoagulant rodenticide "Talon
(Fig. 2). Apart from a narrow zone of bare rock
50 WB" (ICI New Zealand Ltd), a wax briquette
around the coast, both islands are covered with peaty
formulation containing brodifacoum. The need for
soils which are deepest on the ridges and spurs, and
this action became clear during biological surveys of
less than 1 m deep in most other areas.
islands in Doubtful, Breaksea and Dusky Sounds
A vegetation survey in October 1986 showed that
undertaken by Ecology Division and Fiordland
the forest was tallest (6-18 m) towards the centre of
National Park staff in the period 1974-84. These
Hawea Island and was dominated by southern rata
surveys disclosed that Norway rats were plentiful on
(Metrosideros umbellata) and kamahi
(Weinmannia
Breaksea Island and also present on neighbouring
racemosa), with pigeonwood
(Hedycarya arborea),
Hawea Island, that rats had not yet reached adjacent
stinkwood
(Coprosma foetidissima), kotukutuku
Wairaki Island - only 300 m away, and that these
(Fuchsia excorticata), five-finger
(Pseudopanax
three islands (Fig. 1) had high conservation potential
colensoi), miro
(Prumnopitys ferruginea) and
(Thomas, 1985), being among the very few in
broadleaf
(Griselinia littoralis). In less sheltered parts,
Fiordland that remained free of stoats
Mustela
where the canopy was lower, the main species were
erminea (Taylor and Tilley, 1984). Other major
Dracophyllym longifolium, five-finger, southern rata,
objectives of the rat extermination campaign were to
pigeonwood, mapou
(Myrsine australis), kotukutuku,
test our strategy and techniques for rat eradication on
broadleaf, kamahi and stinkwood. The exposed
islands, and to evaluate the feasibility of using these
coastal scrub fringe was mainly of
Dracophyllum,
TAYLOR and THOMAS: RAT ERADICATION ON HAWEA ISLAND
Hebe elliptica, Olearia oporina, O.
reinoldii and flax
mainland is about 900 m which is well in excess of a
rat's swimming range in southern New Zealand waters
In most parts of the forest there was a thick
(Taylor, 1984b).
understory of kiekie
(Freycinetia baueriana), tree ferns
(Dicksonia squarrosa and
Cyathea smithii),
haumakaroa
(Pseudopanax simplex), mahoe
The broad plan of campaign was to observe the rats
(Melicytus ramiflorus), the above-mentioned canopy
and their environment on Hawea Island; to avoid any
species, and other shrubs. The forest floor was
unnecessary actions that would induce food aversion
covered by hen and chicken fern
(Asplenium
or neophobia in the population; to eradicate the rats
bulbiferum), other ferns,
Astelia fragrans, flax, moss,
as quickly as possible using Talon poison; to continue
liverworts and seedlings. In open areas the main plants
monitoring with Talon and non-toxic baits to confirm
were shore tussock
(Poa astonii), Anisotome lyallii,
eradication; and to investigate the potential for using
Carex appressa, moss and lichens.
the same methods to eradicate rats from Breaksea
In 1985 and 1986 the common native forest birds
on Hawea Island were the New Zealand pigeon
(Hemiphaga novaeseelandiae), grey warbler
(Gerygone
Presence, distribution and behaviour of rats
igata), South Island fantail
(Rhipidura fuliginosa
fuliginosa), yellow-breasted tit
(Petroica macrocephala
Observations on the presence, abundance and
macrocephala), bellbird
(Anthornis melanura) and
distribution of rats on Hawea Island before and
silvereye
(Zosterops lateralis). New Zealand falcons
during the poisoning included: recording rat sightings
(Falco novaeseelandiae) were regular visitors.
by day and night; detecting rat tracks on prepared
Common introduced species were the hedgesparrow
surfaces of damp sand; counting rat burrows on three
(Prunella modularis), blackbird
(Turdus merula), song
marked plots (Fig. 3) of 400 m2 at the centre of the
thrush
(Turdus philomelos) and chaffinch
(Fringilla
island, 175 m2 near the south coast, and 675 m2 on
coelebs). Breeding sea birds were southern blue
The Hump; searching for rat burrows, droppings and
penguin
(Eudyptula minor), Fiordland crested penguin
feeding sign; and kill-trapping. Twenty rat snap-traps
(Eudyptes pachyrhynchus), broad-billed prion
were set under sheet metal covers and operated over
(Pachyptila vittata), sooty shearwater
(Puffinus
three nights in both April 1984 and October 1985, and
griseus), and southern great skua
(Stercorarius
73 similar sets were operated over five nights from 17
to 21 April 1986 during the latter part of the
Although the creviced rocky foreshore is a
poisoning campaign. All traps were baited with fish-
suitable habitat for Fiordland skinks
(Leiolopisma
flavoured pet food. The results of the trapping are
acrinasum), none have been found on Hawea Island
expressed as captures per 100 trap nights (C/l00 TN)
(Thomas, 1985). Similarly, large flax weevils
after correcting for unavailable traps by the method of
(Anagotus fairburni) and stag beetles
(Dorcus helmsi)
Nelson and Clark (1973).
appear to be absent, although the dried mandibles of
The behaviour of Norway rats towards bait
a stag beetle were found on The Hump. Fiordland
stations, Talon bait, apples, and to other rats in the
skinks and these large flightless insects are abundant
vicinity of these stations, were observed incidentally
only 300 m away, on adjacent Wairaki Island
during the poisoning operations on Hawea Island.
(Thomas and Taylor, 1988). Presumably, they were
These aspects were also studied in more detail and
exterminated from Hawea Island by rats.
with a variety of non-toxic baits (bacon, cheese,
It is not known exactly when or how Norway rats
bread, shellfish) on Breaksea Island.
reached Hawea Island, but they could well have beenthere for over 150 years. They first arrived at Dusky
The poisoning campaign
Sound, Fiordland, in May 1773 on Captain James
Bird-proof bait stations (illustrated in Thomas and
Cook's
Resolution, and most likely reached Breaksea
Taylor, 1988) were constructed from 100 mm
and Hawea Islands during the intense period of
diameter, yellow, non-perforated, plastic "Nova-coil"
sealing that followed Cook's reports (Thomas and
pipe. Their colour made them easy to find, even
Taylor, 1988). In modern times the risk of re-invasion
among thick ground vegetation. Stations were 400 mm
seems slight. There are no regular anchorages or
long, had a tracking surface of damp sand at each end
landing beaches at Hawea Island, visitors are few and
and a watertight, transparent lid in the centre for
no stores are landed there. The distance to the nearest
inserting and checking the baits. They were heldfirmly to the ground by two hoops of fencing wire.
NEW ZEALAND JOURNAL OF ECOLOGY, VOL. 12, 1989
In March 1986, 73 bait stations were distributed
The eradication of rats from Hawea Island was
over Hawea Island at about 40 m intervals, using a
network of tracks (Fig. 2) which had been cut and
1) Checking all 73 bait stations for interference in
marked during this and a previous visit. To minimise
June, July, August and October 1986, and in
neophobic avoidance by rats, the bait stations were
March and October 1987; old baits in each station
left in position for three weeks before poison was laid
were replaced with two fresh talon baits in July
on 10 April. During the poison operation each station
and October 1986, and March and October 1987.
was baited with two 15 g Talon poison baits, each
2) Snap-trapping at each bait/trap station for a total
containing 0.005% brodifacoum, placed loosely in the
of 2,139 trap nights, from 27 July to 27 August
tunnels and not anchored in any way. The number of
baits taken from each station was recorded, and the
3) Placing ripe eating apples on the ground near
baits replenished daily from 11 to 22 April 1986. At
each of the 73 bait/trap stations for a total of
the end of this period each station was left baited with
3,286 apple nights; from 27 July to 27 August
four Talon baits.
1986, from 24 to 29 October 1986, from 11 to 16March 1987, and from 15 to 21 October 1987.
Between 12 and 24 apples were similarly placedon Breaksea Island during each of these fourvisits to test their acceptability to Norway rats atthose times. Apple is a favoured food of bothNorway and ship rats (pers. obs.) and distinctivegrooves left by the rodents' incisors show upclearly on its skin and flesh. Some birds andinvertebrates will also feed on apples (particularlyover-ripe ones), but their feeding signs are easilydistinguished from that of rats.
4) Searching for fresh sign of rats in locations on
Hawea Island previously favoured by them.
Full details of time and expenditure were recorded
during all operations.
Presence, distribution and behaviour of ratsEleven Norway rats (20/100TN) were trapped near thesouth coast of Hawea Island in April 1984, and eight(14/100TN) in October 1985. On later visits, evidenceof rats was found over the entire island, including TheHump. In March and April 1986, rats were commonlyencountered on Hawea by day and by night, mostoften in the taller forest at the southern end. Ingeneral, rats on Hawea Island were most common inareas with miro trees, areas where ferns and other lowplants covered more than 25% of the ground, andareas close to an extensive intertidal zone. Rat burrowdensity was measured as four per 100 m2 on TheHump, seven per 100 m2 at the centre of the island,and 34 per 100 m2 near the south coast. Thedistribution of rats on the island was also indicated by
Figure 2:
Hawea Island, showing cut tracks and routes, and
the amount of bait removed from each station; most
the distribution of poison-bait and rat-trapping stations
bait was taken from the south end of Hawea Island
and the least from The Hump (Fig. 3).
TAYLOR and THOMAS: RAT ERADICATION ON HAWEA ISLAND
invariably carried away whole, and rats commonlyreturned to the bait stations for the second bait within30 seconds of removing and cacheing the first. Onelarge male rat continued to return as long as thetunnel was re-baited, and took a total of five Talonbaits within approximately 3 minutes. On several lateroccasions, when the observer visited and touched thebait station but did not re-bait it, the rat almostimmediately returned and entered the tunnel.
Large rats carried off whole apples in their
mouths. Smaller rats occasionally rolled apples away,sometimes to burrow entrances, but usually they tookvery large bites and carried off that portion beforequickly returning for further bites. Large pieces ofbread, cheese and bacon were similarly carried off.
Only small crumbs of food were eaten
in situ, whetherin a bait station or in the open.
Large male rats aggressively defended the baited
tunnels from the rest of the population. Althoughsmaller rats were obviously attracted to the smell ofthe baits, they appeared reluctant to approach closerthan about 2-3 m while a large rat was nearby. Somelarge males were seen to chase smaller rats away forperiods of up to two hours, biting any they managedto catch. Finally, when the dominant animals hadapparently had their fill and left, smaller rats(sometimes several at one time) cautiously approachedto feed, and similarly carried off the larger items. Onegroup of three small rats moved around a particular
Figure 3:
Hawea Island, showing the number of nights over
circuit several times, returning to the tunnel every
which poison was taken by rats on different parts of the
15-20 minutes apparently to check for competitors,
island, and the position of plots (stars) on which rat burrows
before they finally carried off the baits.
were counted.
The poisoning campaignThe nightly take of poison bait over the period of theApril 1986 visit is shown in Fig. 4. Eighty-threepercent of the amount available was taken by rats on
Rat tracks were found on the sand surfaces of 13
the first night, and 100% on the second and third
(18%) of the 73 unbaited tunnels left on Hawea Island
nights. Bait take remained above 75% for a total of
for three weeks between March and April 1986.
five nights before dropping to 25% on the sixth night,
During the poisoning campaign on Hawea the rats
and then quickly tailing off. After the seventh night of
almost always removed entire Talon baits from the
poisoning, no more rats were seen on Hawea Island
stations, and it seems from direct observations and
by day or night. Seventy-three snap-traps set adjacent
from lack of residue that fewer than 4% of baits were
to the bait stations during the last five nights of the
eaten
in situ. Despite this behaviour, only six baits or
l2-night operation failed to catch any rats, and
part baits, of a total of 734 poison baits removed by
showed no sign of rat interference. No Talon bait was
rats on Hawea Island, were found on the surface -
taken on the twelfth night of poisoning. However, a
and it is certain that all others were taken into rat
check of all bait stations on 3 June showed that two
burrows before being eaten or stored. During later
more baits had disappeared. These were both from
tests on Breaksea Island, Norway rats were observed
one bait station in an area with many rats, which had
readily entering the Nova-coil tunnels soon after the
previously had more poison taken from it than had
tunnels were positioned and baited. Talon baits were
any other station.
NEW ZEALAND JOURNAL OF ECOLOGY, VOL. 12, 1989
poison, 50 m of Nova-coil pipe, 80 rat snap-traps andassociated materials, and apples for detecting thepresence of rats.
In addition there were travel and servicing costs
associated with moving personnel and gear to andfrom Fiordland (c. $7,300), and the support providedby Fiordland National Park's MV
Renown and hercrew (c. $27,000).
It seems certain that all rats living on Hawea Islandwere killed within a period of about two weeks duringthe April 1986 poisoning, and that the two talon baitsfound missing on 3 June were taken soon after thetwelfth night of poisoning by an already dying rat.
Figure 4:
The percentage of "Talon 50 WB" baits taken
When poisoning started, rats were abundant.
each 24
hours on Hawea Island during the first 13 days ofthe poisoning campaign. Note: A total of 146
baits was
They were commonly seen during day and night, there
available each day.
were many fresh burrows, and the Talon baits weretaken promptly from most tunnels. A comparison of
Eradication of the rats from Hawea Island was
the numbers kill-trapped on Hawea Island (20 and
confirmed as follows:
14/100 TN in April 1984 and October 1985
1) Although Talon baits were continually available
respectively), with kill-trapping results and population
in the 73 bait stations, none were touched by rats
densities based on live trapping on three other islands
in a total of 36,866 bait-station nights between
Ö Motuhoropapa Island (9.1/100 TN and 2.6-4.2
June 1986 and October 1987.
rats/ha; Moors, 1985b), Whale Island (6.9 and
2) No rats were caught, or traps interferred with by
23.6/100 TN and 5-10 rats/ha; Bettesworth, 1972),
rats, during 2,139 snap-trap nights between April
and Campbell Island (14.6/100 TN and 10 rats/ha;
and July 1986.
Taylor, 1986) - indicates that the Hawea population
3) No apples were touched by rats during a total of
may have been in the order of 6-11 rats/ha, i.e., a
3,286 apple nights in July, August and October
total population of around 50-100 rats.
1986, and in March and October 1987. Eighty-
Our observations of Norway rats at bait stations
eight percent of apples placed on Breaksea Island
on Hawea and Breaksea Islands showed that Talon
during these same visits were eaten by rats either
was extremely attractive and palatable to them. Their
within a few hours, or during the first four nights
behaviour of removing and cacheing baits (Calhoun,
after being put out - giving an overall take of 70
1962), and chasing away subordinate rats, means that
per 100 apple nights.
not all rats will have ready access to the bait during
4) No fresh sign of rats has been found on any of
the first few days of a poisoning operation.
ten visits to Hawea Island between April 1986 and
Each 15 g Talon bait contains 0.005%
brodifacoum, and it has been estimated that at thisconcentration the LD ,. for Norway rats weighing 250
g is 1.3 g of bait (Dubock and Kaukeinen, 1978). One
The campaign involved a total of 154 person-days
bait, therefore, is sufficient to kill several rats.
(PD) by paid and volunteer workers. This included
However, since rats die several days after they have
time spent in planning and organising (23 PD),
consumed a lethal dose of brodifacoum, each rat
travelling (51 PD), surveying and tracking (29 PD),
poisoned on Hawea would almost certainly have eaten
constructing bait stations (8 PD), poisoning (27 PD),
several baits. Norway rats can die within 4 days of
checking results on two follow-up visits (3 PD), and
consuming large doses of brodifacoum, whereas those
analysing data and writing two preliminary reports (13
that have ingested little more than a lethal dose may
PD). The total cost in salaries and wages was about
live for up to 12 days (Dubock and Kaukeinen, 1978).
As a consequence, it would be expected that dominant
Major material costs were less than $700 at 1986
individuals would eat a surfeit of bait and die in the
values, and comprised the cost of 32.5 kg of Talon
first 3-4 days. Less dominant rats should then
TAYLOR and THOMAS: RAT ERADICATION ON HAWEA ISLAND
successively gain access to bait, whether in bait
were habituated to the poison baits but were avoiding
stations or in caches down burrows, and be poisoned
fish-flavoured baits on newly-positioned snap-traps set
over the following two or three weeks, until all are
near each bait station. It seems likely that dying rats
could become dependent on Talon, from bait stations
The 100 mm diameter bait stations were an ideal
or from caches in burrows, for food. If so, they
size for Norway rats. As expected, their yellow colour
would spend little time in the open, and thus reduce
seemed not to concern the rats, which are colour
the risk of secondary poisoning to other species. The
blind. In fact, rats are thought to prefer yellows and
main avian predators at risk on Hawea Island were
greens - which would appear to them as light grey -
southern great skuas and New Zealand falcons, both
to many other colours (Brooks and Rowe, 1987).
of which may have preyed on live but poisoned rats.
The approximate 40 m spacing between bait
However, comparable numbers of both species were
stations used on Hawea Island followed Moors
seen in the vicinity of Hawea Island before and after
(1985a), and was based on the average distance
the poisoning, and there was no evidence of adverse
between successive captures found for marked Norway
rats on Motuhoropapa Island - 113 m for males and
The major advances of the Hawea Island
49 m for females. However, the mean range-length
operation' over most previous rat control or
was about 200 m for five male and 108 m for three
eradication exercises in New Zealand were:
female rats trapped at least twice on forested
1) Eradication was achieved in one continuous short
Motuhoropapa Island (Moors, 1985b), and about 232
operation - not in irregular bursts.
m for three male and 141 m for five female Norway
2) The poison used had no history of inducing food
rats similarly studied in forest on Stewart Island
aversion in rats eating a sub-lethal dose.
(Hickson, Moller and Garrick, 1986). For several
3) Only one poisoning technique was used, and its
species of rodents, including house mice
(Mus
effectiveness could therefore be fully evaluated.
musculus), ship rats, and Norway rats (Fitzgerald,
4) All poison bait was presented in standard
Karl and Moller, 1981; Innes and Skipworth, 1983;
amounts at fixed points, and checked and
Taylor, 1986), it has been shown that surviving
replaced daily - this not only allowed accurate
animals quickly expand their home ranges once their
data to be gathered on the amount of poison
neighbours are removed. These studies, of home range
taken by rats, and for the progress of the
and swift invasion response, suggest that the spacing
campaign to be continuously monitored, but also
of bait stations on Hawea Island was unnecessarily
for the rats to learn where food was regularly
The possibility of accidental exposure of non-
5) The bait stations used were bird proof and with
target species to primary or secondary poisoning was
an entrance of sufficient diameter to be readily
closely monitored during the poisoning campaign, but
accepted by Norway rats.
no such deaths were identified. Insects are not known
6) The Talon baits, each containing well in excess of
to be directly affected from feeding on Talon. Bird
a lethal dose, could be readily removed by rats to
species vary widely in their susceptibility to
eat in a secure place of their own choice. This will
brodifacoum, and a secondary hazard can also exist to
have increased the chance of a lethal dose being
insectivorous birds and raptors feeding on prey that
consumed quickly. With oats, wheat or similar
has consumed poison bait (Godfrey, 1985). There was
baits, rodents are forced to frequently visit, or
no evidence that birds were interfering with the poison
stay in, the bait stations while they feed on
inside the bait stations, and the rats scattered very
little about outside. On Hawea Island, some baits that
7) An excellent method was devised for detecting the
were left in the tunnels for long periods showed
presence of rats, by laying out whole apples. This
evidence of being chewed by insects, probably wetas,
simple technique is an important advance in
cockroaches and ants.
helping to decide if rats have reached an island,
Despite careful searches, no poisoned rats were
or if eradication has been achieved.
found on the surface of Hawea Island, and it appears
8) The possibility of neophobia causing some rats to
that most died in their burrows or under thick cover.
avoid Talon baits was countered by setting out
Our snap-trap results near the end of the Hawea
the bait stations three weeks prior to poisoning,
poisoning operation suggest that the last surviving
and keeping Talon constantly available to rats at
rats, all of which would already have eaten Talon,
the same sites for over two years.
NEW ZEALAND JOURNAL OF ECOLOGY, VOL. 12, 1989
9) No special effort, or perseverance, was needed to
choice of diet, rats that have smelled a particular food
get the last rat.
on another rat's breath prefer it, even to other more
10) Only 154 person-days were required to complete
familiar foods. Similarly, rats that have acquired an
the eradication campaign and to confirm its
aversion to a food will eat it again after exposure to
success, at a total cost of about $65,000.
conspecifics that have recently fed on it (Galef, 1987;
There is no simple way to predict the costs of
Galef, Mischinger and Malenfant, 1987). It seems
eradication campaigns on other offshore islands,
sensible, therefore, to take advantage of this
especially those outside of Fiordland, by extrapolation
knowledge and keep poison baits continually available
from the Hawea Island operation. Some work was
at regular sites during eradication campaigns against
developmental and will not need repetition. Cost/area
Norway rat populations using second-generation
relationships will be roughtly linear for materials, but
anticoagulants. Thus at any moment during the course
the cost of transport, accommodation, and wages will
of a campaign, a large proportion of the living
rapidly decrease per unit area with increasing island
population will have consumed some poison and will
size. These items will also vary immensely according to
be influencing the remaining population to eat it, even
circumstances, such as the location, topography, and
though temporarily they may be chasing some less
vegetative cover of the island, and facilities already
dominant individuals away. It is likely that other
species of rats and house mice are also susceptible to
We consider that the "pulse baiting" technique,
similar "peer pressure" (Galef and Clark, 1971; Galef
of replacing poison baits every five to seven days
and Heiber, 1976; Bean, Galef and Mason, 1988).
(Dubock, 1979) or the "minimal baiting" system
We therefore recommend that whenever possible
(Richards and Huson, 1985) usually recommended for
all poison baits be checked and replaced daily, during
economy and safety when controlling rodent
rodent eradication campaigns using anticoagulant
populations with second-generation anticoagulants, are
poison. For each island being poisoned, a knowledge
not appropriate when attempting eradication of rats
of the fauna likely to be at risk, appropriately
from an island. In these circumstances the main
designed bait stations, and the shortest possible
priorities are to kill every rat, and to guard against
poisoning campaign are important safeguards against
any possibility of encouraging the development of bait
primary or secondary poisoning of non-target species.
shyness, poison resistance, or neophobia in the
Any extra financial costs, through more bait being
taken by the rats, will be more than offset by the
We detected little evidence of neophobia in this
lower cost of a much shorter and more predictable
population of Norway rats which has possibly been
isolated from the selective pressures of human control
The rodent extermination strategy and techniques,
for over 150 years. Moors (1985a, 1986) claimed that
as developed on Hawea Island and described in this
Norway rats are more wary of man-made objects,
paper, were applied with only slight modifications on
such as traps and bait stations, than are ship rats or
Breaksea Island in May/June 1988. We are currently
kiore. However, we know of no evidence supporting
part way through the planned campaign, but five visits
this generalisation from uninhabited islands or remote
to the island since July 1988 have failed to detect any
parts of New Zealand. A lack of severe neophobia
sign of surviving rats.
could well be a common characteristic of long-isolated
populations of commensal rats (Mitchell, Kirschbaum
Thanks are due to: the former Department of Lands
and Perry, 1975; Cowan, 1977). Therefore, we suggest
and Survey, the Fiordland National Park Board, and
that the best chance of eradicating any species of rat
the Department of Conservation for financial and
from New Zealand's island reserves is when the job,
logistic assistance; Lance Shaw, Peter Brotherston,
once started, is completed quickly before the
Ruth Dalley, Bob Walker, and other members of the
population is affected by the human-induced phobias
crew of MV
Renown for their active support and
that have frustrated many control operations
companionship; Ron and Robynne Peacock for their
generous hospitality at Te Anau; and Kim Morrison,
Behavioural research has shown that Norway rats
Paul Green, Richard Sadleir, Graeme Taylor, Ian
rely on their peers in deciding where and what to eat.
Flux, Jane Maxwell, Sandra Higgins, Phil Denny, Ron
When given a choice of feeding locations they prefer
Peacock, Robynne Peacock, Colin McColl, Shaunagh
sites that conspecifics are exploiting (Shorten, 1954;
O'Halloran, Mick Clout, and others who assisted at
Barnett, 1975; Galef and Heiber, 1976). When given a
TAYLOR and THOMAS: RAT ERADICATION ON HAWEA ISLAND
various times in the field. Jocelyn Tilley drew the
Elton, c.; Ranson, R.M. 1954. Containers for baiting.
maps and graph, and Jacqueline Beggs and Mike
In: Chitty, D. (Editor),
Control of rats and mice,
Fitzgerald commented on the manuscript.
Vol. 1, pp. 147-159. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
305 p.
Fitzgerald, B.M.; Karl, B.J.; Moller, H. 1981. Spatial
organization and ecology of a sparse population
Atkinson, I.A.E. 1986. Rodents on New Zealand's
of house mice
(Mus musculus) in a New Zealand
northern offshore islands: distribution, effects
forest.
Journal of Animal Ecology 50: 489-518.
and precautions against further spread.
In:
Galef, B.G. 1987. Social influences on the
Wright, A.E.; Beever, R.E. (Editors),
The
identification of toxic foods by Norway rats.
offshore islands of northern New Zealand, pp.
Animal Learning and Behaviour 15: 327-332.
13-40. New Zealand Department of Lands and
Galef, B.G.; Clark, M.M. 1971. Social factors in the
Survey Information Series 16. 255 p.
poison avoidance and feeding behaviour of wild
Barnett, S.A. 1975.
The rat: a study in behaviour.
and domesticated rats.
Journal of Comparative
Chicago University Press, Chicago. 318 p.
and Physiological Psychology 75: 341-357.
Bean, N.J.; Galef, B.G.; Mason, J.R. 1988. The
Galef, B.G.; Heiber, L. 1976. The role of residual
effects of carbon disulfide on food consumption
olfactory cues in the determination of feeding site
by house mice.
Journal of Wildlife Management
selection and exploration patterns of domestic
52: 502-507.
rats.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Bettesworth, D.J. 1972 (unpublished).
Aspects of the
Psychology 90: 727-739.
ecology of Rattus norvegicus
on Whale Island,
Galef, B.G.; Mischinger, A.; Malenfant, S.A. 1987.
Bay of Plenty, New Zealand. M.Sc. Thesis,
Hungry rats' following of conspecifics to food
University of Auckland. 67 p.
depends on the diets eaten by potential leaders.
Bremner, A.G.; Butcher, C.F.; Patterson, G.B. 1984.
Animal Behaviour 35: 1234-1239.
The density of indigenous invertebrates on three
Godfrey, M.E.R. 1985. Non-target and secondary
islands in Breaksea Sound, Fiordland, in relation
poisoning hazards of "second generation"
to the distribution of introduced mammals.
anticoagulants.
Acta Zoologica Pennica 173:
Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand 14:
Greaves, J .H. 1985. The present status of resistance to
Brooks, J.E.; Rowe, F.P. 1987.
Commensal rodent
anticoagulants.
Acta Zoologica Pennica 173:
control. Vector control series. World Health
Organisation, Geneva, VBC/87.949. 102 p.
Hickson, R.E.; Moller, H.; Garrick, A.S. 1986.
Calhoun, J.D. 1962.
The ecology and sociology of the
Poisoning rats on Stewart Island.
New Zealand
Norway rat. United States Department of Health,
Journal of Ecology 9: 111-121.
Education and Welfare. Public Health Service
Howard, W.E.; Marsh, R.E.; Palmateer, S.D. 1973.
Publication 1088, Washington DC. 288 p.
Selective breeding of rats for resistance to sodium
Chitty, D. 1954. The study of the brown rat and its
monofluoroacetate.
Journal of Applied Ecology
control by poison.
In: Chitty, D. (Editor),
10: 731-736.
Control of rats and mice, Vol. 1, pp. 160-305.
Innes, J.G.; Skipworth, J.P. 1983. Home ranges of
Clarendon Press, Oxford. 305 p.
ship rats in a small New Zealand forest as
Cowan, P.E. 1977. Neophobia and neophilia: new-
revealed by trapping and tracking.
New Zealand
object and new-place reactions of three
Rattus
Journal of Zoology 10: 99-110.
species.
Journal of Comparative and
Merton, D. V. 1978. Controlling introduced predators
Physiological Psychology 91: 63-71.
and competitors on islands.
In: Temple, S.A.
Dubock, A.C. 1979. Alternative strategies for safety
(Editor),
Endangered birds: management
and efficacy of rodenticides.
Proceedings of the
techniques for preserving threatened species, pp.
5th British Pest Control Conference, Stratford
121-128. University of Wisconsin Press, Madison.
Upon-A von: Session 5,
paper 14. 15 p.
Dubock, A.C.; Kaukeinen, D.E. 1978. Brodifacoum
Mitchell, D.; Kirschbaum, E.H.; Perry, R.L. 1975.
(Talon rodenticide), a novel concept.
Proceedings
Effects of neophobia and habituation on the
of the 8th Vertebrate Pest Conference,
poison-induced avoidance of exteroceptive stimuli
Sacremento, California: 127-137.
NEW ZEALAND JOURNAL OF ECOLOGY, VOL. 12, 1989
in the rat.
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Taylor, R.H., 1984b. Distribution and interactions of
Animal Behaviour Processes 104: 47-55.
introduced rodents and carnivores in New
Moors, P.J. 1985a. Eradication campaigns against
Zealand.
Acta Zoologica Fennica 172: 103-105.
Rattus norvegicus on the Noises Islands, New
Taylor, G.A. 1986 (unpublished).
The ecology of
Zealand, using brodifacoum and 1080.
In: Moors,
Norway rats on Campbell Island. Ecology
P.J. (Editor),
Conservation of island birds, pp.
Division report, D.S.I.R. 169 p.
143-155. ICBP Technical Publication 3. 271 p.
Taylor, R.H.; Thomas, B.W. 1986 (unpublished).
Moors, P.J. 1985b. Norway rats
(Rattus norvegicus)
Second progress report on a rat eradication
on the Noises and Motukawao Islands, Hauraki
project at Breaksea Sound, Fiordland National
Gulf, New Zealand.
New Zealand Journal of
Park. Ecology Division report, D.S.I.R. 14 p.
Ecology 8: 37-54.
Taylor, R.H.; Thomas, B.W.; Taylor, G.A. 1986
Moors, P.J. 1986. Eradication campaigns against
(unpublished).
Preliminary report on a rat
Norway rats on the Noises Islands, Hauraki Gulf.
eradication project at Breaksea Sound, Fiordland
In: Wright, A.E.; Beever, R.E. (Editors),
The
National Park. Ecology Division report, D.S.I.R.
offshore islands of northern New Zealand, p.
246. New Zealand Department of Lands and
Taylor, R.H.; Tilley, J.A.V. 1984. Stoats
(Mustela
Survey Information Series 16. 255 p.
erminea) on Adele and Fisherman Islands, Abel
Nelson, L.; Clark, F.W. 1973. Correction for sprung
Tasman National Park, and other offshore
traps in catch-effort calculations of trapping
islands in New Zealand.
New Zealand Journal of
results.
Journal of Mammalogy 54: 295-298.
Ecology 7: 139-145.
Redfern, R.; Gill, J.E.; Hadler, M.R. 1976.
Thomas, B.W. 1985. Observations on the Fiordland
Laboratory evaluation of WBA 8119 as a
skink
(Leiolopisma acrinasum Hardy).
In: Grigg,
rodenticide for use against warfarin-resistant and
G.; Shine, R.; Ehmann, H. (Editors),
The
non-resistant rats and mice.
Journal of Hygiene,
biology of Australasian frogs and reptiles, pp.
Cambridge 77: 419-426.
17-22. Royal Zoological Society of New South
Richards, C.G.J.; Huson, L.W. 1985. Towards the
Wales. 527 p.
optimal use of anticoagulant rodenticides.
Acta
Thomas, B.W.; Taylor, R.H. 1988. Rat eradication in
Zoologica Fennica 173: 155-157.
Breaksea Sound.
Forest and Bird 19: 30-34.
Rzoska, J. 1953. Bait shyness, a study in rat
Towns, D. 1988. Rodent eradication from islands -
behaviour.
British Journal of Animal Behaviour
the conservation potential.
Forest and Bird 19:
1: 128-135.
Shorten, M. 1954. The reaction of the brown rat
Wace, N.M. 1986. The rat problem on oceanic islands
towards changes in its environment.
In: Chitty,
- research is needed.
Oryx 20: 79-86.
D. (Editor),
Control of rats and mice, Vol. 2, pp.
Yaldwyn, J.C. 1978. Chairman's summing up during
307-334. Clarendon Press, Oxford. 532 p.
"General Discussion on Part 3".
In: Dingwall,
Taylor, D.P. 1984a (unpublished).
The identification
P.R.; Atkinson, I.A.E.; Hay, C. (Editors),
The
and detection of the rats of New Zealand and the
ecology and control of rodents in New Zealand
eradication of ship rats on Tawhitinui Island.
nature reserves, p. 237. New Zealand Department
Dissertation for diploma in Parks and Recreation,
of Lands and Survey Information Series 4. 237 p.
Lincoln College, Canterbury. 73 p.
Source: http://newzealandecology.org.nz/system/files/articles/NZJEcol12_23.pdf
Main Rupatadine References Contents: Main Rupatadine References Pharmacodynamics / Pharmacokinetics Dual effect of a new compound, rupatadine, on edema induced by platelet-activating factor and histamine in dogs: Comparison with antihistamines and PAF antagonists. Queralt M, Merlos M, Giral M, Puigdemont A. Drug Dev Res 1996; 39 (1): 12-8. The antihistamine-H1 and antiplatelet activating factor (PAF) activities of seven compounds, including rupatadine, a new antiallergic drug, were studied in healthy beagle dogs using a new experimental model that allows simultaneous testing of PAF and histamine reactions in the same animal. The method was based on the measurement of wheal area induced in dogs' skin by intradermal injection of PAF (1.5 mug) or histamine (2.5 mug). Rupatadine and the H1-antihistamine drugs cetirizine, levocabastine, and loratadine, administered orally at doses of 1 or 10 mg/kg showed similar maximum potencies (75-85% of wheal inhibition) 4-8 h after treatment. Levocabastine was the longest-acting compound (55% and 69% inhibition 24 h after administration of 1 or 10 mg/kg, respectively). Rupatadine, loratadine, and cetirizine behaved similarly, showing 34% and 58% inhibition at 24 h at the same doses. Dual PAF and histamine antagonist SCH-37370 exhibited mild anti-H1 activity, the maximum effect being 27% at 10 mg/kg. Pure PAF antagonists WEB-2086 and SR-27417 showed no effect against histamine-induced wheals. Only rupatadine, SR-27417A, SCH-37370, and WEB-2086 showed PAF antagonist activity, whereas pure antihistamines were inactive. The most potent PAF antagonist was SR-27417A, with a maximum effect of 56% and 80% at 1 and 10 mg/kg, respectively. Rupatadine and WEB-2086 antagonized PAF-induced wheal response, although they showed less maximum effect and shorter duration of action than SR-27417A. SCH-37370 exhibited only slight PAF antagonist activity at 10mg/kg. Overall, the histamine- and PAF-induced wheal model in dogs proved useful for independent evaluation of histamine and PAF antagonist properties of the tested compounds, as pure antagonists blocked the effect of only one of the mediators. Rupatadine was the only one of the seven compounds studied that showed potent dual activity against PAF and histamine. Protective effect of rupatadine fumarate in experimental conjunctivitis in guinea pigs. Ferrando R, Giral M, Balsa MD, Merlos M, Garcia Rafanell J, Forn J. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 1996; 18 (Suppl B): 140. XX Congress of the Spanish Society of Pharmacology and the IV Spanish-French Meeting on Pharmacology. Granada (Spain), September 18-20 1996. The topical antiallergic activity of the novel histamine (H) and PAF antagonist rupatadine fumarate (RF; UR-12592 fumarate) eyedrops was evaluated in comparison with loratidine (LOR) in a model of H-, PAF-or ovalbumin (OVA)-induced conjunctivitis in guinea pigs. From the results it was concluded that RF could be useful in the topical treatment of allergic conjunctivitis. (conference abstract). Conjunctivitis was induced by topical application of H (400 ug) or PAF (10 ug) in naive animals or OVA (140 ug) in actively sensitized guinea pigs. Drugs were administered as eye-drops (20 ul) 15 min before agonist or antigen provocation. Inflammation was scored (0-10 point scale) at 5, 15, 30, 60, 90, 120, and 150 min after induction. RF (0.001-0.01 % w/v) strongly and dose-dependently inhibited H-induced conjunctivitis, being about 20-fold more potent than LOR (IC50 values at 30 min were 0.0015 and 0.034% for RF and LOR, respectively). RF (0.05-0.2%) also inhibited PAF-and OVA-induced conjunctivitis, e g. mean scores (at 30 min), PAF: 6.8 and 4.2 for control and 0.1% RF, respectively; OVA: 7.2 and 3.8. LOR, at the same concentrations, inhibited OVA-, but not PAF-induced conjunctivitis.
R ESEARCH ARTICLE The effect of mirtazapine on methotrexate-inducedtoxicity in rat liver Bunyami Ozogula, Abdullah Kisaoglua, Mehmet Ibrahim Turanb,∗, Durdu Altunerc, Ebru Senerd,Nihal Cetine, Cengiz Ozturke,f a Department of Surgery, Faculty of Medicine, Ataturk University, 25240, Erzurum, Turkeyb Department of Paediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, Ataturk University, 25240, Erzurum, Turkeyc Department of Pharmacology, Faculty of Medicine, Recep Tayyip Erdogan University, 53100, Rize, Turkeyd Department of Pathology, Erzurum Region Education and Research Hospital, Erzurum, Turkeye Department of Pharmacology-Toxicology, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Ataturk University, 25240,