Doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.007
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and
the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links
and possible mechanisms. A review
Bea R.H. Van den Bergha,*, Eduard J.H. Mulderb, Maarten Mennesa,c, Vivette Gloverd
aDepartment of Developmental Psychology, Catholic University of Leuven (KULeuven), Tiensestraat 102, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
bDepartment of Perinatology and Gynaecology, University Medical Center Utrecht, Lundlaan 6, 3584 EA, Utrecht, The Netherlands
cDepartment of Paediatric Neurology, University Hospital Leuven (KULeuven), Herestraat 49, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
dInstitute of Reproductive and Developmental Biology, Imperial College London. Du Cane Road, London W12 0NN, UK
A direct link between antenatal maternal mood and fetal behaviour, as observed by ultrasound from 27 to 28 weeks of gestation onwards, is
well established. Moreover, 14 independent prospective studies have shown a link between antenatal maternal anxiety/stress and cognitive,behavioural, and emotional problems in the child. This link generally persisted after controlling for post-natal maternal mood and otherrelevant confounders in the pre- and post-natal periods. Although some inconsistencies remain, the results in general support a fetalprogramming hypothesis. Several gestational ages have been reported to be vulnerable to the long-term effects of antenatal anxiety/stress anddifferent mechanisms are likely to operate at different stages. Possible underlying mechanisms are just starting to be explored. Cortisolappears to cross the placenta and thus may affect the fetus and disturb ongoing developmental processes. The development of the HPA-axis,limbic system, and the prefrontal cortex are likely to be affected by antenatal maternal stress and anxiety. The magnitude of the long-termeffects of antenatal maternal anxiety/stress on the child is substantial. Programs to reduce maternal stress in pregnancy are thereforewarranted.
q 2005 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Keywords: Pregnancy; Stress; Programming; Cortisol; Fetal behaviour; Child behaviour; Developmental neuroscience; Review
2.2. Antenatal maternal stress and anxiety and fetal behaviour on ultrasound observation . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3. The short and long term links between anxiety/stress during pregnancy and the development of the child . . . . . . . .
* Corresponding author. Tel.: C32 16 32 58 60; fax: C32 16 32 60 55.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (B.R.H. Van
0149-7634/$ - see front matter q 2005 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.007
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
3.5. Effects of antenatal maternal depression, a co-morbid symptom of anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.6. Effects of antenatal anxiety/stress on handedness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
4. Two physiological mechanisms by which the maternal affective state may affect the fetus in humans . . . . . . . . . .
5. Stress hormones and the developing fetal nervous system: how are they related to behavioural/emotional regulation
neurotransmitter systems, and disturbed behaviour inanimal offspring A consistent finding in the non-human primate work is that stressing the mother during
‘And surely we are all out of the computation of our age,
pregnancy has a long-term adverse effect on attention span,
and every man is some months elder than he bethinks him;
neuromotor behaviour, and adaptiveness in novel and stress-
for we live, move, have a being, and are subject to the
inducing situations (e.g. enhanced anxiety) of the offspring
actions of the elements, and the malices of diseases, in that
other World, the truest Microcosm, the Womb of our
Human studies on the long-term effects of prenatal stress
Mother'(Sir Thomas Browne, Religio Medici, 1642)
are difficult. In 1893, Dr Alfred W. Wallace (cited in )wrote to Nature: ‘Changes in mode of life and in intellectual
The question of the importance of prenatal environmen-
occupation are so frequent among all classes, that materials
tal factors for development, behaviour and health, has been
must exist for determining whether such changes during the
scientifically studied from the 1940s onwards in humans
prenatal period have any influence on the character of the
and even earlier, from the 19th century onwards, in
offspring' (p. 3). Joffe concluded that, in human
experimental embryology (see The fetal program-
studies, obtaining sufficient control of genetic and post-natal
ming hypothesis states that the environment in utero can
environmental factors had been the major difficulty to
alter the development of the fetus during particular sensitive
enable the post-natal behavioural differences under inves-
periods, with a permanent effect on the phenotype. In recent
tigation to be attributed conclusively to prenatal variables.
years, the work of Barker has given a great impetus to
However, he concluded that even if uncertainty about
research in this particular field. He proposed "the fetal
etiological relationships exists, human studies provide
origins of adult disease hypothesis". This states that the
sufficient evidence to enable preventive action to be
physiological, neuroendocrine or metabolic adaptations that
initiated with regard to a variety of childhood disorders,
enable the fetus to adapt to changes in the early life
without waiting for the methodological issues to be
environment result in a permanent programming (or re-
unraveled. ‘though the action may be more effective
programming) of the developmental pattern of proliferation
when they are' p. 308).
and differentiation events within key tissues and organ
In humans, studies during the last two decades have
systems and can have pathological consequences in later life
provided continuing and mounting evidence that negative
The key observation on which this was based was that
maternal emotions during pregnancy are associated with an
weight at birth was a strong risk factor for coronary heart
adverse pregnancy outcome. The association between high
disease, diabetes mellitus, and obesity later in life. This
antenatal anxiety/stress and preterm delivery and low birth
finding has been reproduced in many independent studies,
weight for gestational age are the most replicated findings
although it appears to be the ponderal index rather than birth
and have been discussed fully elsewhere (for recent reviews
weight that matters (for reviews see for coronary heart
see A meta-analysis of 29 studies on work-
disease; for obesity). Most of the work on the possible
related stress and adverse pregnancy outcome showed that
mechanisms underlying these findings have focused on
occupational exposures significantly associated with pre-
nutrition, although there is also evidence that the hypo-
term birth included physically demanding work, prolonged
thalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA)-axis may be involved
standing, shift and night work, and a high cumulative work
In parallel with this work in humans there has been a
fatigue score. Physically demanding work was also related
strong body of animal research linking prenatal stress and
to pregnancy-induced hypertension and preeclampsia .
Pregnancy-induced hypertension was shown to be related to
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
Trait Anxiety score (and maternal ponderal index) during
the 7th month of pregnancy Hypertension and
Criteria to define episodes of each of four fetal behavioural states
preeclampsia in turn, increase the rate of preterm delivery
Behavioural state
and small-for-gestational-age infants Hansen et al.
have shown that severe life events during pregnancy
Heart rate pattern
increased the frequency of cranial–neural-crest malfor-
mations in the child. Unexpected death of a child during the
first trimester was associated with adjusted odds ratios of 8.4
(2.4–29.0) for cranial–neural-crest malformations and 3.6
States 1F and 2F are also called quiet sleep and active sleep, respectively;
(1.3–10.3) for other malformations.
states 3F and 4F, quiet wakefulness and active wakefulness, respectively
In this paper, we review studies of the past two decades,
a HRP A is a stable heart rate with a narrow oscillation bandwidth; HRP
concurrently or prospectively studying the link between
B has a wider oscillation bandwidth with frequent accelerations during
antenatal maternal anxiety/stress on the one hand, and fetal
movements; HRP C is stable (no accelerations), but with a wider oscillation
behaviour and later development of the child on the other
bandwidth than HRP A; HRP D is unstable, with large, long-lasting
hand. Evidence for underlying physiological mechanisms in
accelerations that are frequently fused into sustained tachycardia. If none of
humans and possible effects of stress hormones on prenatal
these combinations are met this is called no-coincidence (NoC) orindeterminate state.
brain development are also reviewed. More specifically, thequestion is raised whether maternal anxiety, apart fromaffecting the HPA-axis and limbic system may alsoaffect the development of the prefrontal cortex, which is
characterize stable temporal organisation near term
presumed to underlie behavioural alterations seen in
Four distinct fetal states can be identified based on
children of mothers who were highly anxious/stressed
specific associations between the three variables mentioned
during pregnancy. Finally, we formulate some suggestions
(see legend to for descriptions). Although some
for strengthening further research.
level of temporal organization is already present at 28–30weeks, behavioural state organization progressively devel-ops between 30 and 40 weeks, both in utero and in low-risk
2. Antenatal maternal stress and anxiety
preterm born infants This developmental pattern,
and the human fetus
which parallels particular aspects of brain development, ischaracterized by a gradual increase in quiet sleep and awake
Reports from the pre-ultrasound era, both anecdotal and
states, and a profound decrease in indeterminate state, a
semi-scientific (i.e. non-controlled), have suggested that
gradual decrease over time in body movements and basal
prenatal maternal stress, anxiety, and emotions affect fetal
FHR, and an increase in FHR variability and fetal move-
functioning, as evidenced by increased fetal heart rate
ment-FHR coupling (i.e. FHR accelerations associated with
(FHR) and motility Ultrasound techniques, enabling
body movements) Besides macro-analysis of
FHR monitoring and direct fetal behaviour observation for
behavioural state organization, i.e. calculation of the % of
prolonged periods of time, have for two decades been used
time spent in each state, basal FHR, its variability, and the %
in longitudinal and cross-sectional studies of the effects of
incidence of body movements during episodes of states 1F
antenatal maternal anxiety and stress. Both observational
and 2F (see are often calculated (micro-analysis) to
and stress/emotion-induced study designs have been
identify state-specific characteristics.
employed and the results will be reviewed here. The results
Fetal behavioural states can be regarded as precursors of
can only be understood in the context of some background
the adult sleep–wake states. Fetal and adult sleep states not
information on normal fetal neurobehavioural development
only share comparable features of non-REM/REM, cardi-
ovascular, respiratory, and (probably) metabolic control, butalso share the neuronal substrate, neurotransmitters, and
2.1. Normal development of human fetal behaviour
receptors that are believed to underlie sleep control fromearly in fetal life onward
A number of distinct fetal movement patterns has been
Recent studies on adult animals and humans have
distinguished, emerging at a well described time point
elucidated that the cyclic alternation between non-
during the first 15 weeks of gestation (post-menstrual age),
REM/REM states and wakefulness is a highly regulated
including body movements, breathing movements, hiccups,
and arm, leg, head, and mouth movements As
Several neuronal networks involving distinct mesopon-
pregnancy progresses, rest–activity cycles become increas-
tine and hypothalamic brain areas and a variety of excitatory
ingly linked to specific fetal heart rate patterns and to
and inhibitory neurotransmitters, -modulators, and -peptides
absence and presence of rapid eye movements (REM),
have been found to form an intricate web of interactions
respectively. These cycles finally develop into ultradian
underlying sleep–wake control (for detailed reviews see
fetal behavioural states (sleep–wake cycles), which
Each behavioural state is now believed to result
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
from a specific balance between activities of wake-
macro- and/or micro-analyses for recordings that lasted at
promoting and sleep-promoting neurons and the activities
of many neurotransmitter systems (cholinergic, noradren-
Three studies that have evaluated the immediate
ergic, serotonergic, GABA-ergic).
relationship between maternal anxiety/stress and fetal
Processes during sleep have been found to be intimately
behaviour in the first half of pregnancy found no
related to memory and cognition in adult awake state
observable effect on spontaneous motor activity (nos. 10–
Disturbed sleep–wake organization is a characteristic of
12). Four out of the five independent studies with a
neurological and psychopathological diseases (e.g. ADHD,
comparable study design (nos. 2–4, 9, 11) have reported
autism, depression, schizophrenia). At least for some of these,
evidence of increased arousal in near-term fetuses of high
exposure to prenatal maternal stress has been suggested as a
stress/anxious women, as reflected by an increase in fetal
causative factor. The sleep and stress control systems share
wakefulness, increased FHR variability and % of body
particular brain loci, such as the locus coeruleus and forebrain
movements during active (REM) sleep and state 4F, and a
centres. This brings us to the question of whether there are
decrease in the amount of quiet (non-REM) sleep. The
observable, objective effects of gestational stress on the
results of DiPietro et al. can be generally viewed to be in
developing human fetus. If so, which features of fetal
accordance with these findings, although no information is
behavioural development and organization are being affected,
provided as to which fetal functional aspect was specifi-
cally involved. In two studies (nos. 7, 8) they showed
there differential effects on the fetus between different types of
overall increased % of body movements and FHR
maternal stress, and which mechanisms may be involved?
variability and accelerations (at 36 weeks in particular) infetuses whose mothers reported higher levels of perceived
2.2. Antenatal maternal stress and anxiety and fetal
stress and emotions, more pregnancy-related hassles, and a
behaviour on ultrasound observation
negative valence toward pregnancy. Results from earlierreports (nos. 5, 6), i.e. reduced FHR variability and poorer
An overview of the results obtained in 12 observational
movement-FHR coupling in fetuses of women with high
studies on the relationship between prenatal maternal
perceived stress, seem to be different from the later
psychological states and fetal behavioural development is
findings of this group. Of particular interest are the
presented in All studies involved uncomplicated
observations that fetuses of women with a positive vs.
pregnancies, and healthy pregnant women (mainly nullipar-
negative attitude toward pregnancy exhibit different overall
ous) and their newborns. The studies were also uniform
levels of motor activity (reduced versus increased,
regarding the demographic background of the participants,
respectively). As positive (pleasant) emotions and negative
the majority being Caucasian, well-educated, and of middle
stressors are believed to have similar physiological effects
SES-class. Maternal age, the number of participants, and
(on the fetus), their observations deserve to be replicated in
fetal recording length on the other hand, varied largely
other studies.
among the studies. Most studies controlled for the possible
The findings for maternal anxiety/stress on fetal
effect of circadian rhythms and meals, and some also
performance are in line with the well-known report on
adjusted for potential confounders, including maternal age,SES, smoking, and alcohol intake. The levels of maternal
hyperkinetic fetuses of acutely stressed women during an
anxiety and stress were assessed by using self-administered
earthquake (no. 1), but are opposite to those described by
questionnaires, which are either widely used and validated
Groome et al. for unknown reasons (no. 4). Their sample
or developed by the authors. The Spielberger State Trait
consisted for nearly 50% of black women, and fetuses of
Anxiety Inventory (STAI was used most frequently
black women have been described to spend more time in
among the studies. It differentiates between current feelings
quiet sleep than white fetuses As these data were not
of tension and apprehension (state anxiety) and an
analysed by race, it remains unclear whether this con-
individual's relatively stable anxiety-proneness (trait
founder was a factor of importance with respect to the
anxiety). Some studies used measures of general stress,
mentioned discrepancy in findings.
involving either stress-provoking (daily hassles, life events)
One study has reported that stress experienced in early
or stress-resulting aspects (stress appraisal, perceived
pregnancy had an observable effect on fetal behaviour as
stress). Pregnancy-specific anxiety and affect were included
early as at 28 weeks (no. 11). Only a few studies have
in two studies (nos. 7, 8; ). Similar definitions of fetal
focused explicitly upon the timing of gestational stress (nos.
movement patterns and behavioural organization (when
3, 11). They have suggested that maternal anxiety/stress
appropriate) were used across the studies, and fetal move-
experienced during early pregnancy, but also during later
ments were observed and registered by a researcher, except
stages of pregnancy, are associated with the above-
for the studies by DiPietro et al. (nos. 5–8). These authors
mentioned fetal effects near term. The latter results suggest
used an ultrasound device for automated detection of fetal
that maternal anxiety/stress-related mechanisms might
motility (actograph) and analysed the 50-min records for
affect the fetal nervous system during the first two trimesters
total observation time only. Other groups provided results of
of pregnancy. However, possible alterations have only been
Table 2Ultrasound studies of the effect of prenatal maternal stress and anxiety on fetal behaviour
Ianniruberto 1981
Qualitative description: "panic stricken"
Fetal hyperkinesia for 2–8 h, followed
women during earthquake
by a 24–72 h period of reduced motility
GA: 18–36 wkRL:–
Positive correlation between state
Administered on day of recording
anxiety and %FM (during total rec. time
Age: 26 (19–31) yr
and during S2F-4F);
No effect of induced maternal emotions
Negative correlation between state anx.
Nulliparous: 100%
State scale administered on day of
(T3) and trait anx. (T1,T2,T3) and
Age: 24 (20–28) yr
State and Trait scales at 12–22 wk (T1)
Positive correlation between state anx.
23–31 (T2) and 32–40 wk (T3)
and %S4F and %FM (during total rec.
time and during states 2F-4F)
Positive correlation between state and
Administered 3 days before fetal
trait anx. and %S1F;
Negative correlation between state and
trait anx. and %FM during state 2F
Daily hassles (general) and uplifts
Greater perceived stress was associated
expressed as one score (ratio) of
FHR: mean FHR and variability (SD)
with reduced FHR variability;
age: 29 (22–36) yr
perceived stress/stress appraisal; infor-
GA: 20–40 wk, 6 times at 4–wk interval
No reported effects on %FM and % state
mation over past 24 h
RL: 50 min/session
Daily hassles (general) and uplifts
Higher reported stress was associated
expressed as one score (ratio) of
FHR: baseline FHR FHR-FM coupling
with less FHR-FM coupling
Age: 29 (22–36) yr
perceived stress/stress appraisal; infor-
GA: 20–40 wk, 6 times at 4–wk interval
mation over past 24 h
RL: 50 min/session
(1) intensity of experienced emotions
Increased %FM and tendency toward
FHR: # accelerations
more FHR accelerations in women who
(2) daily (general) stressors (perceived
GA: 24, 30, 36 wk
were more hassled or negative about
RL: 50 min/session
their pregnancy (higher intensity of
(3) pregnancy-specific daily hassles and
hassles relative to uplifts) and who
uplifts (frequency, intensity, ratio has-
reported more daily stressors;decreased
%FM in women with high emotional
(4) composite Z-score
intensity, but only for women in low-SES class
(continued on next page)
Table 2 (continued)
(1) intensity of experienced emotions
Decreased FHR at 36 wk in women who
FHR: mean FHR and variability (SD)
showed high emotional intensity;
Age: 30 (21–39) yr
(2) daily (general) stressors (perceived
GA: 24, 30, 36 wk
Increased FHR variability at 36 wk in
RL: 50 min/session
women who had higher frequency of
(3) pregnancy-specific daily hassles and
uplifts (frequency, intensity, ratio has-
Increased %FM in women who reported
greater emotional intensity, appraised
(4) composite Z-score
their daily lives as more stressful, and
who had more pregnancy-specific has-sles and a more negative valence toward
pregnancy;Decreased %FM in women who per-
ceived their pregnancy to be more
intensely and frequently uplifting andwho had a positive emotional valencetoward pregnancy
Sjo¨stro¨m 2002
High anxiety group: tendency toward
Nulliparous: 100%
Administered about 2 wk before fetal
FHR: basal FHR and variability (esti-
more %HRP-C (state anx.) and %HRP-
Age: 26 (SD 4) yr
recording; the state anx. scale was
mated from paper chart)
Median split analysis
D (state and trait anx.); tendency toward
considered to reflect perceived anxiety
lower FHR variability in episodes of
between 25 and 36 wk
HRPs A and B (state anx.); lower FHRin HRP-C and increased FHR variability
in HRP-D (state and trait anx.); positive
correlation between state/trait anx. and
%HRP-D;No effect of anxiety on %FM in each ofthe distinct fetal states
No significant relationships between
Administered on day of recording
state or trait anxiety and %FM or other
fetal movement patterns
STAI: state anx. scale before fetal
FM: observer (T1–T3)
High numbers of LE and DH reported at
Nulliparous: 100%
recording;Life events (LE) and daily
FHR: basal FHR and FHR variab.
T1 were not related to %FM at T1, but
Age: 31 (17–45) yr
hassles (DH): frequencies reported over
were sign. associated with increased
GA: 15–17 wk, 27–28 wk, 37–39 wk
Analysis: high-low con-
%FM and FHR variability during
Administered at 15–17wk (T1),
RL: 60 min (T1, T2) and 120 min (T3)
trasts (scores OP75 vs !
episodes of HRP-B (S2F) at both T2 and
27–28 wk (T2), and 37–39 wk (T3)
P25) and correlational
T3, and, at T3, with an increase in%HRP-D (%S4F), a decrease in %HRP-A (%S1F) and a decrease in %NoC;Fetuses of high-stress women exhibitedbetter state organization;No sign. effects of state/trait anxiety atT1–T3 on the near-term fetus
Self-constructed questionnaire adminis-
FM: arm, leg, head movements
No relationship between maternal stress
Low-risk population
tered just before fetal observation
scores and the numbers of fetal arm, leg,
and head movements
–information not provided or not applicable (e.g. FHR at early gestation, !24 wk); %FM: incidence of fetal (gross) body movements, expressed as % of time; FHR: fetal heart rate; HRP: fetal heart rate pattern; S1F-4F:fetal behavioural states 1F through 4F; %NoC: incidence of no-coincidence of state parameters (% of time); GA: gestational age; RL: record length; micro: micro-analysis of %FM and/or FHR and its variability duringepisodes of HRPs A–D or states 1F–4F.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
observed so far with ultrasound from 28 weeks of gestation
3. The short and long term links between anxiety/stress
during pregnancy and the development of the child
A number of studies have recently investigated the
effects of induced maternal stress, emotions, and hormonal
3.1. Overview of results
changes on fetal functioning Changes in fetal heartrate and motility that occurred during a maternal cognitive
Evidence from earlier studies has been largely incon-
challenge (arithmetic test or the Stroop colour-word
clusive but more recent methodologically improved
matching test) were compared with values obtained during
studies support the notion of an overall relationship
pre and post-test periods. The whole procedure was
between negative maternal emotions during pregnancy
completed within about 15 min. The observed effects during
and reproductive outcome . The intensity and chronicity
testing compared with baseline were usually statistically
(or duration) of antenatal anxiety/stress and lack of
significant but small, e.g. a 10% decrease in fetal movement
appropriate coping mechanisms have been identified as
and a 5 bpm increase in fetal heart rate .
critical factors A recent review suggests that
The results of this kind of experiments are clearly of
antenatal maternal stress results in a general susceptibilityto psychopathology .
interest but have to be viewed with some caution because
We here review published or ‘in press' prospective
of potential methodological pitfalls. As pointed out above,
studies from the past 20 years, in which the assessment of
the human fetus exhibits a large amount of spontaneous
maternal anxiety/stress was started during pregnancy
body movements occurring at a rate of about 0.4–5 per
(The 17 studies-14 independent, one two-wave
min . Body movements are associated with FHR
study (nos. 11, 14), and one three-wave study (nos. 6, 16,
accelerations, such that it may increase from 130 to 160–
17)—all with a different design are summarized. Studies are
170 bpm within a few seconds. Finally, fetal behaviour is
ordered by the age of the child at final assessment.
organized in rest–activity or sleep–wake cycles. Both
In general, the studies show that antenatal maternal
physiological variables and responsiveness to external
anxiety/stress was positively related to regulation problems
stimuli depend on the state the fetus is in (input–output
at the cognitive, behavioural, and emotional levels. These
state relationship). Thus, for successful testing fetal
problems were assessed either by behavioural observations
responses to elicited maternal psychological challenges,
or recordings (nos. 1–6, 8–10, 16, 17), and/or by teachers'
stimulus-free control periods of the same duration as that
ratings (nos. 13, 15, 16), and/or by mother's ratings (nos. 4,
of the test procedure are required. These control periods
6–8, 11–16).
must be obtained from the same fetus during a
In newborn babies, regulation problems were expressed
comparable behavioural state In the only controlled
in less good scores for the Brazelton Neonatal Assessment
(counterbalanced) study in this field to date (no. 2), the
Scale (nos. 1, 9), neurological examination (no. 2), cardiac
effect of induced emotion on fetal performance was
vagal tone (no. 3) and behavioral states (no. 6).
studied by showing a film of a normal delivery to
Infants were rated by an observer as having less good
pregnant women at term during the second half hour of a
interactions with their mother (no. 4), being highly reactive
2-h fetal behaviour recording. Although this film evoked
(no. 5), worse regulation of attention (no. 8) and having
intense maternal emotions (some women were crying
poorer language abilities (no. 10), and by their mother as
when watching) and a positive correlation was found
having sleeping, feeding and activity problems (no. 6), and
between maternal state anxiety and fetal body movements,
being irritable and difficult (nos. 6–8). Scores on the Bayley
no differences in movement incidence and behavioural
Scales of Infant Development were worse at 8 and 24 m
state distribution were revealed when comparing data of
(nos. 8–10), but not at 7 m (no. 6).
the experimental day with comparable data on a control
Pre-school children and children were rated by their
day when no maternal emotions were induced. Further
mother (nos. 11–16), teachers (nos. 15, 16), an external
understanding of immediate maternal–fetal interactions
observer (no. 16) or themselves (no. 16) as showing poorer
awaits future studies that take into account the
attention, hyperactivity, behavioral and emotional pro-
peculiarities of fetal behaviour.
blems, and they were rated by their teacher has having
To conclude, a link between antenatal maternal mood
low school grades and bad behaviour (no. 13).
and ultrasonographically observed fetal behaviour is well
Finally, adolescents showed impulsive behaviour when
established. Although two studies showed that maternal
performing computerized cognitive tasks and scored
anxiety/stress measured at 12–21 and 15–17 weeks
lower on intelligence subtests (no. 17). Unpublishedresults of Obel et al. (personal communication,
influenced near term fetal behaviour, an immediate link
indicate that stressful life events increased the risk for
has in general only been observed from 27 to 28 weeks of
ADHD problems in pre-adolescents (9–11-year-olds).
pregnancy onwards. The mechanisms underlying these links
Unpublished results of Van den Bergh et al. confirm
are presently obscure.
a link between high antenatal anxiety and behavioural
Table 3Prospective studies on the effect of prenatal maternal anxiety and stress on postnatal behavioural developmenta
Anxiety/stress measure in preg-
Outcome assessment:
Statistical analyses:
Impact of antenatal anxiety/stress:
Size at outcome, characteristics of
Child's age at outcome; gender;
Method; confounders controlled
Negative child outcome (normal
Timing; questionnaires; physio-
measures; observer
letter); positive and zero effect
!20 wk; 30–34 wk
Higher total distress score associ-
Age: 31 (18–40) yr
Total distress score based on: Trier
Neonatal Behavior Assessment
Controlled for: gestational age
ated with more infant regulation
No obstetrical or psychiatric path-
Inventory for the Assessment of
(NBAS), by observer
(Medical record data on birth)
problems on NBAS (e.g. alertness,
Chronic Stress, Prenatal Distress
cost of attention, state regu-
Singleton pregnancy
Questionnaire, Perceived Stress
Higher basal cortisol levels at
Life Experience Scale
30–34 wk related to more infant
Morning cortisol: saliva samples
difficulties in habituating to new or
! 20 wk, 30–34 wk
Linear and logistic regression
Moderate to severe stress associ-
70 most stressed versus 50 non-
Questionnaire about life events,
Controlled for: maternal age,
ated with lower birth weight,
stressed (from cohort)
conditions at work (e.g., fatigue,
Head circumference
gestational age, educational level,
smaller head circumference and
chemicals), smoking, alcohol,
Prechtl's neurological obser-
social support, smoking, alcohol,
lower Prechtl's neurological score
vation, by external observer
tranquillizers, gender of child
Singleton pregnancy
General Health Questionnaire
(Prechtl's Obstetric Optimality
%16 wk; 32–34 wk
Birth, 1 day, 3–4 wk
Correlations; regression
Higher negative TE at %16 wk,
Nulliparous: 100%
Negative trait emotionality (TE)
Medical record data (e.g. Apgar
associated with higher neonate
Age: 17.3 (13–19) yr
based on: State Trait Anxiety
1', 5'; risk factors at birth and
Apgar 50 and lower cardiac vagal
No obstetrical risk or psychiatric
Inventory (STAI)-trait, State Trait
24 h; no. of resuscitation methods
Anger Scale (STAS)-trait, and
Higher negative SE at 32–34 wk
NEO-AC Personality Inventory
Cardiac vagal tone at 3–4 wk (data
associated with more abnormal-
depression, anxiety and hostility
analyzed from 100 infant resting
ities on the newborn profile
EKG according to Porges'
Social support mediated effect
Negative state emotionality (SE)
between TE at %16 wk and
based on: STAI-state, STAS-state,
cardiac vagal tone
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
Higher cortisol at %16 wk
Inventory of Socially Supportive
associated with lower neonate
Apgar 10, 50 and increased need for
Saliva cortisol: 5 samples at
resuscitation at birth
20 min intervals at
No effect of SE at %16 wk, TE at
32 wk, cortisol at 32–34 wk onmeasurs of infant outcome orcardiac vagal tone
High pregnancy risk index group
Pregnancy risk index (scale of
100 face-to-face play interactions
had high postnatal maternal scores
Braverman and Roux on demo-
(videotape), by external observer
on BDI, STAI and Locus of
No obstetrical risk
graphic characteristics, stress,
Colorado Child Temperament
Inventory, by mother
Depressed mothers have less opti-mal interactions (e.g. infant lessrelaxed, more fussiness, moredrowsy state) and rate their infantas being more emotional
Correlations; hierarchical linear
Higher antenatal anxiety and
STAI-state anxiety
12 girls, 10 boys
depression related to higher infant
Age: 28 (18–36) yr
Center for Epidemiological
Harvard Infant Behavioral Reac-
Controlled for: anxiety and
negative behavioral reactivity
No psychiatric risksingleton preg-
Studies Depression Inventory
tivity Protocol (videotape), by
depression 8 wk after birth
external observer
(Medical record data on medicalrisk and birth)
12–22 wk; 23–31 wk; 32–40 wk
Correlations; LISREL
Higher antenatal state and trait
Nulliparous: 100%
Prechtl's neurological observation
Controlled for: postnatal anxiety at
anxiety related to: more activity in
(Important Life Event Scale, Daily
(1 wk) by external observer; 2 h
state 2–4 and more crying at 1 wk;
No obstetrical risk or psychiatric
Hassles Scale, Coping Scale,
behavioral state observation
(Educational level, smoking, birth
more difficult temperament at
Social Support Scale, Pregnancy
(1 wk) by observer
weight for gestational age, gender
10 wk and 7 m; more irregularity
Feeding score and mother-infant
of child, Prechtl's Obstetric
in feeding and sleeping, more
interaction during feeding (1 wk;
Optimality Score)
activity at 7 m.
10 wk), by external observer
No effect of anxiety on Prechtl's
Behavioral ratings (1 wk; 7 m),
neurological score, feeding score,
ITQ (10 wk; 7 m), ICQ (7 m), by
(Unpublished result: higher social
BSID (7 m), by observer
support and expression of
emotions associated with higher
infant MDI and PDI)
Near 21 wk; 35 wk
Mothers of infants with difficult
Nulliparous: 100%
ITQ-Revised, by mother
temp. had higher STAI anxiety
Personality Research Form
scores at 21 and 35 wk, were more
Self-esteem (Epstein scale)
defendant and impulsive, have less
self-esteem than mothers of
infants with easy temp.
NZ35–100 (study 4)
Correlations; t-tests
Mothers of infants with difficult
Institute for Personality Assess-
ITQ-Revised, by mother
(Maternal age, Apgar score, edu-
temp. had higher IPAT-anxiety
ment and Testing (IPAT) anxiety
cation, parity, gender of infant,
and less optimal CPI personality
No obstetrical risk or psychiatric
scale; California Personality
length of labour, birth weight)
scores during pregnancy than
Inventory; (CPI); McGill Pain
mothers of infants with easy temp.
Maternal characteristics correlated
Cortisol, ACTH, b-endorphin:
with b-endorphin from placental
maternal and placental blood
blood sample (only 4 of 120 tests
samples at 26–34 wk, during early
Mothers of difficult infants hadlower levels of b-endorphin during
later stages of labor (only 1 of 15tests significant)
(continued on next page)
Table 3 (continued)
Anxiety/stress measure in preg-
Outcome assessment:
Statistical analyses:
Impact of antenatal anxiety/stress:
Size at outcome, characteristics of
Child's age at outcome; gender;
Method; confounders controlled
Negative child outcome (normal
Timing; questionnaires; physio-
measures; observer
letter); positive and zero effect
15–17 wk; 27–28 wk; 37–38 wk
10 days; 3 m; 8 m
Correlation; logistic regression;
Higher fear of giving birth and
Nulliparous: 100%
86 girls, 84 boys
having handicapped child at
Pregnancy Related Anxieties
BSID and IBR (3 m, 8 m), by
Controlled for: postnatal perceived
15–17 wk associated with more
No obstetrical risk
external observer
stress and depression at 3 m, 8 m,
Perceived Stress Scale
ICQ (3 m, 8 m) by mother (total
educational level, smoking, alco-
problems at 3 and 8 m
Singleton pregnancy
(Trait Anxiety, depression
score for adaptational problems
hol use, gender, breastfeeding)
Higher perceived stress at 15–
and difficult behavior)
(SES, birth weight, gestational age
17 wk associated with more diffi-
Saliva cortisol: 7 samples every
at birth, obstetric risk, GHQ)
cult infant behavior at 3 m and 8 m
2 h starting at 8 a.m, at 15–17 wk,
and infant attention-regulation
27–28 wk, 37–38 wk
More daily hassles at 15–17 wkassociated with lower infant MDI
Higher fear of giving birth at 27–
28 wk related to lower infant MDIand PDI at 8 mHigh early morning salivary cor-tisol at 37–38 wk associated with
lower infant MDI at 3 m and PDI
No effects of daily hassles onattention regulation and difficultbehavior
c2; linear regression;
Higher anxiety associated with
52 girls, 53 boys
Controlled for: gender child, edu-
lower score on orientation cluster
Age: 30.4 (21–38) yr
NBAS (3 wk), by observer
cational level, birth weight, type of
of NBAS at 3 wk and lower MDI
No medical pathologysingleton
BSID and IBR (1 and 2 yr), by
feeding, parity, HOME-subscale,
alcohol, smoking during preg-
c2 (without control for confoun-
nancy, postnatal maternal anxiety
der); high anxiety associated with
and depression symptoms
lower scores on task orientationand motor co-ordination on the
IBR at 12 m, and lower MDI andPDI at 12 m and 24 m
1–3 m; 4–6 m; 7–9 m (within 6 m
Correlations; hierarchical linear
More severe objective stress
after ice storm, in many cases
BSID-Mental scale by observer
exposure associated with lower
Age: 30.6 (20–42) yr
during pregnancy)
MacArthur Communicative
Controlled for: birth weight, gen-
MDI and lower productive and
Objective stress measure of dis-
Development Inventory (French
der, month of gestation, age at
receptive language abilities on
aster; treat, loss, scope and change
MacArthur Inventory; effects on
Subjective stress measure: Impact
(SES, pregnancy and birth com-
MDI only significant for stress
of Event Scale Revised
plications, postpartum depression
during first six months of preg-
nancySubjective stress measure notrelated to MDI or languageabilities
NZ7447 (from cohort)
Logistic regressioncontrolled for:
High levels of anxiety at 32 wk
Anxiety items of the Crown-Crisp
3595 girls, 3853 boys
timing of prenatal anxiety, birth
associated with more inattention/
Age: 28 (14–46) yr
Strengths and Difficulties Ques-
weight for gestational age, mode
hyperactivity and emotional pro-
tionnaire (SDQ), by mother
of delivery, parity, smoking, alco-
blems in boys and with more
hol, SES, maternal age, postnatal
emotional and conduct problems
anxiety and depression (EDPS)
in girlsHigh levels of anxiety at 18 wkassociated with more emotionalproblems in girls
NZ527–1297 (6 m) and NZ389–
1–16 wk; 17–28 wk; 29–40 wk
Correlations; latent variable path
Psychological distress modestly
900 (5 yr) (from cohort)
Self-construct pregnancy ques-
50% male (6 m) 54% male (5 yr)
related to negative temperament at
tionnaire on psychological distress
ITQ and Preschool Temperament
Controlled for: somatic illness,
6 m.; strongest for psychological
(anxiety/depression and mood
Questionnaire (adapted), by
nausea, maternal age
distress at 1–16 wk
Higher psychological distress at
1–16 wk related to higher negativeemotionality at 5 yr; strongest for
More risks during pregnancy
Infant temperament questionnaire
associated with lower school
(self-construct), by mother (6 m)
grades and more negative behavior
School grades and marks for
in school at 6 yr
behavior in school, by two tea-chers (6 yr)
Z6204–6493 (from cohort)
Logistic regression
High levels of anxiety at 32 wk
Anxiety items of the Crown-Crisp
3000 girls, 3204 boys
Controlled for: timing of prenatal
associated with more behavioural/
Age: 28 (14–46) yr
anxiety, birth weight for gesta-
emotional problems in both boys
tional age, mode of delivery,
parity, smoking, alcohol, SES,
High levels of anxiety at 18 wk
maternal age, postnatal anxiety
associated with more behavioural/
and depression (EDPS)
emotional problems in girls (effectof 18 wk stronger than effect of
10; 12; 20; 28; 32; 36 wk
Correlations, linear and logistic
High stress and heavy smoking
Swedish 10-item version of Per-
146 girls, 142 boys
independently associated with
ceived Stress Scale
18 symptoms (DSM-IV criteria for
Controlled for: smoking, timing of
more ADHD symptoms; fulfill-
ADHD), by mother and teacher
stress and smoking, maternal edu-
ment of diagnostic criteria for
Impact item of the SDQ, by
cation and civil status, presence
ADHD related to prenatal stress
and salary of father figure
Week 10 accounted for the largestportion of the variance
(continued on next page)
Table 3 (continued)
Anxiety/stress measure in preg-
Outcome assessment:
Statistical analyses:
Impact of antenatal anxiety/stress:
Size at outcome, characteristics of
Child's age at outcome; gender;
Method; confounders controlled
Negative child outcome (normal
Timing; questionnaires; physio-
measures; observer
letter); positive and zero effect
NZ71 (72 children)
12–22 wk; 23–31 wk; 32–40 wk
Correlations, hierarchical linear
Higher anxiety at 12–22 wk
Nulliparous: 100%
STAI-state anxiety
34 girls, 38 boys
associated with more ADHD
Composite score for ADHD
Controlled for: timing of prenatal
symptoms and externalizing pro-
No medical or psychiatric pathol-
symptoms, externalizing and
anxiety, postnatal trait anxiety,
blems and with higher self report
internalizing problems based on:
educational level, smoking, birth
CBCL, by mother and teacher;
weight for gestational age, gender
Anxiety at 32–40 wk not a signifi-
Conners' Abbreviated Teacher
cant independent predictor of
Rating Scale, by mother and
(Prechtl's Obstetric Optimality
childhood disorders
teacher; Groninger Behaviour
Observation Scale, by external
observerSTAIC, by child
12–22 wk; 23–31 wk, 32–40 wk
Correlations; MANCOVA's
High state anxiety at 12–22 wk is
Nulliparous: 100%
28 girls, 29 boys
Controlled for: timing of prenatal
related to impulsive cognitive
Performance of child on compu-
anxiety, postnatal trait anxiety
style (reacting faster but making
No medical or psychiatric pathol-
terized Encoding Task and Stop
(Educational level, birth weight
more errors) in the Encoding task
for gestational age, smoking, Pre-
and to lower scores on the intelli-
Vocabulary and Block Design of
chtl's Obstetric Optimality Score)
gence subtests, but not to Stop
Wisc-R intelligence test
Task performance.
No effect of trait anxiety and no
effect of state anxiety at 23–31 and32–40 wk on encoding, Stop Task,or intelligence subtests
wk, week(s); m, month(s); ACTH, adrenocorticotrophic hormone; ADHD, Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; SES, Socio-Economic Status; temp., temperament. Abbreviation of Scales: BDI, Beck
Depression Inventory; BSID, Bayley Scales of Infant Development; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; EPDS, Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; GHQ, General Health Questionnaire; IBR, Infant BehavioralRecords; ICQ, Infant Characteristics Questionnaire; ITQ, Infant Temperament Questionnaire; MDI, Mental Developmental Index; NBAS, Neonatal Behavior Assessment Scale; PDI, PsychomotorDevelopmental Index; STAI, State Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAIC, State Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; aAll studies in this table are prospective follow-up
studies of the period 1985–2004. Under the heading ‘Sample‘ characteristics of the mothers are given out of which eligibility criteria can be inferred. Under the headings ‘Anxiety/stress measures in pregnancy'
and ‘Outcome assessment' those variables are given that were reported in the articles (between brackets: variables not used in the statistical analyses). Under the heading ‘Statistical analyses' the variables arelisted that were controlled for in the described statistical analysis method (between brackets: confounders not used in the statistical analyses). Under the heading ‘Impact of antenatal anxiety/stress' only thosenegative, positive and zero effects are presented that were reported in the article.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
disorders measured with the Child Behavior Checklist up
child behaviour persisted even after controlling for potential
to 14–15 years of age.
confounders in the pre and/or post-natal period, lendssupport to the idea that fetal programming by antenatal
3.2. Controlling for the effect of confounders
anxiety/stress is occurring in humans, as in the animalmodels. It is likely that the effects of the changed prenatal
It is important to ask whether the good evidence for a link
environment interact with genetic factors in defining the
between antenatal maternal anxiety/stress and regulation
phenotype at birth . Those studies, which have
problems in the child, also implies fetal programming
examined the same sample at two or more times, show
induced directly by maternal anxiety/stress. The link may be
the same effects persisting with the same magnitude over 3
mediated by other prenatal or post-natal environmental
(nos. 11, 14) and 9 years (nos. 6, 16). Although more
factors, such as smoking during pregnancy or post-natal
research is needed to study the potential modulating effect
maternal anxiety, or may be explained by rater bias. There
of other post-natal factors than post-natal mood (e.g.
may also be a genetic vulnerability passed directly from
attachment and parenting style) , all these long-
mother to child. The underlying mechanism is likely to be a
term results again support a prenatal programming
prenatal programming one if the link can be shown to be
specifically with antenatal and not post-natal anxiety/stress,if it cannot be explained by rater bias, and if the link persists
3.3. Timing of gestational stress
after controlling for the effect of other prenatal environ-mental factors. Several studies have attempted to control forthese confounders.
Studies are inconsistent with regard to the gestational age
For measuring anxiety or stress during pregnancy all
at which the effects of antenatal maternal anxiety/stress are
studies used mother's self rating of symptoms or events,
most pronounced. Rodriguez and Bohlin (no. 15;
rather than a clinical diagnosis. Studies 1, 3, 7, and 8 also
concluded that stress at week 10 accounted for the largest
included stress hormone measures (Some studies
proportion of the variance in ADHD-symptoms at age 7, and
have analyzed specific pregnancy anxieties (no. 8) or the
Martin et al. (no. 12) found the strongest effect on negative
number of life events and/or appraisal of recently experi-
emotionality in 5-years-old for psychological distress
enced life events (nos. 1, 2, 8) or disaster (no. 10) during
during the first three months of pregnancy. Laplante et al.
pregnancy, which indicates that the anxiety and stress are
(no. 10) found that high levels of objective stress exposure
likely to be more specific to the antenatal period. Most other
(measured within 6 m after an ice storm) affected intellec-
studies used standardized scales (nos. 3, 5–11, and 14–17)
tual capacities at age 2 only when the stress occurred in the
or assembled a scale (nos. 4, 12, 13) to measure perceived
first six months of pregnancy. Van den Bergh (nos. 16, 17)
anxiety and stress confined to the prenatal period. As the
found that effects on childhood disorders at age 8–9 and
perception of anxiety in pre- and post-natal periods are
cognitive functioning at age 14–15 were confined to
significantly correlated associations found
maternal anxiety at 12–22 weeks of pregnancy. Huizink
between antenatal anxiety/stress and child's outcome can
and colleagues (no. 8) found more pronounced effects for
be spurious. However, studies nos. 5, 6, 8, 9, 11, 14, 16, and
maternal anxiety/stress at 15–17 weeks and pregnancy-
17 used a multivariate analysis including measures of
specific anxieties at 27–28 weeks, while early morning
perceived post-natal anxiety and/or depression and/or stress
cortisol levels at 37–38 weeks had a small effect. O'Connor
as confounding variables, and still found strong links
et al. (nos. 11, 14) found that anxiety at week 32 was a
between antenatal maternal anxiety and regulation problems
stronger predictor of behavioural/emotional problems at age
in the child.
4 and 7 than anxiety at 18 weeks.
Studies nos. 2, 11, and 14 have used large numbers, which
The fact that several gestational ages have been reported
gives a good opportunity to not only control for post-natal but
to be vulnerable to the long-term effects of antenatal
also for antenatal confounding variables, e.g. for educational
anxiety/stress may indicate that different mechanisms are
level and income, smoking, parity, birth weight, gestational
operating at different stages. However, observed differences
age, and gender of the child. The other studies, using smaller
in effects of timing may also be due to differences between
numbers, controlled in their statistical analyses at least for
the studies, including the scales used for dependent and
confounders shown in their own sample to be influential (nos.
independent variables (see the exact timing of the
1, 5–10, 12, 15–17). Moreover, potential confounders were
anxiety measurements, the time period to which they refer,
also controlled by using strict eligibility criteria, e.g. for
as well as to the intensity of anxiety and the actual
parity, age, medical, obstetrical and psychiatric risks (see
persistence of anxiety throughout pregnancy In
nos. 1, 3, 5–9, 16, 17). Only study nos. 3, 4, 7, and 13, and one
addition, genetic differences and differences in psychologi-
report of study no. 6 showed insufficient control for
cal, medical–obstetrical, and environmental factors con-
confounders in their design or statistical analyses.
trolled for and not controlled for might be relevant
We can conclude that the fact that in most studies the link
This is clearly an area that needs more attention
between antenatal maternal emotions and later infant or
in future research.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
3.4. Magnitude of the effect
epinephrine levels and not to the higher rates of low birthweight and prematurity Zuckerman et al. observed
It is important to assess the amount of variance in
that babies of women with depressive symptoms (NZ1123)
outcome that may be related to antenatal maternal emotions.
cried excessively at 8–72 h after birth and were difficult to
Several of the studies show associations large enough to be
console; no effects were found on neurological state.
of clinical significance (nos. 10, 11, 14–16; ). For
Dawson and colleagues have found that during mother–
example, in study no. 10, maternal stress exposure to an ice
infant interaction, children of depressed mothers showed
storm at 0–12 weeks and 13–24 weeks of pregnancy
increased autonomic arousal (higher than normal heart rates
explained 27.5 and 41.1% of the variance in the Bayley MDI
and cortisol levels), and reduced activity in brain regions
scores at age 2, respectively. In studies nos. 11 and 14, being
that mediate positive approach behaviour The authors
in the top 15% for antenatal anxiety at 32 weeks of
indicate that there is suggestive evidence from their follow-
gestation, approximately doubled the risk for having a son
up study (NZ159 at 13–15 m; partial follow-up to 42 m
with ADHD symptoms at age 4 and 7, even after allowing
) that the post-natal experience with the mother had
for a wide range of covariates including post-natal anxiety
more effect on infant frontal EEG than prenatal factors.
up to 33 m. Study no. 6 indicates that maternal anxiety at
O' Connor et al. examined antenatal depression as
12–22 weeks explained 15 and 22% of the variance in
well as anxiety, using the self-rating Edinburgh Post-natal
externalizing problems and ADHD symptoms at age 8–9,
Depression Scale antenatally as well as post-natally.
respectively. Other studies show more modest effects. In
Antenatal depression had a somewhat weaker effect on
study no. 8, for instance, 3–8% of the variance in
child outcome than antenatal anxiety. When both were used
behavioural regulation and mental and motor development
together in a multivariate analysis, the effects of antenatal
at 3 and 8 m was explained, mainly by specific anxiety/
anxiety were apparent but not those of antenatal depression.
stress at 15–17 and 27–28 weeks of gestation , and
In contrast, the effects of post-natal depression were found
no effect of state or trait anxiety during these periods was
to be separate but additive to those of antenatal anxiety .
Ma¨ki et al. in a prospective epidemiological study
Differences in the amount of explained variance may be
(NZ12,059), found that in the male offspring of antenatally
related to the timing of anxiety/stress (see above) or to a
depressed mothers there was a significant but only slight
difference in the degree of anxiety/stress experienced by the
increase in criminality.
pregnant women across the different studies. For instance, instudy no. 8, mean state anxiety was 32.9 (SDZ7.8) at 15–17
3.6. Effects of antenatal anxiety/stress on handedness
weeks and 31.1 (SDZ8.4) at 37–38 weeks of gestation These values equal decile 4, thus below the mean, of a
Studies that looked at handedness have shown
Dutch female norm population In study no. 6, mean
that antenatal life events or anxiety are associated with a
state anxiety in comparable gestation periods was 38.7
greater incidence of mixed handedness in the child. This
(SDZ7.7) and 36.1 (SDZ8.8), equaling decile 6 and decile
was defined as the child using either hand for a range of task
5 of the same norm population, respectively.
such as drawing or throwing a ball. While in itself not abehavioural problem, mixed handedness has been shown to
3.5. Effects of antenatal maternal depression, a co-morbid
be associated with a range of neurodevelopmental problems
symptom of anxiety
such as dyslexia, autism, and ADHD. This mild adverseeffect would again fit with the animal research in which a
Much more research has been done on the effects of
wide range of disturbances have been found in the offspring,
antenatal anxiety than depression, although it is well
including a disturbance of laterality .
established that there is a strong co-morbidity between thetwo Field's group has performed a range of studies on
3.7. Weaknesses of the studies
the outcome for the newborn baby with mothers who weredepressed during pregnancy . They showed that
One weakness of many or most of the studies concerns
maternal depression during pregnancy was significantly
the outcome measures. Researchers did not use specific
associated with less than optimal scores on many subscales
marker tasks for testing specific cognitive functions (e.g.
of the Brazelton Neonatal Assessment Scale (e.g. habitu-
attention, inhibition, working memory, processing speed).
ation, orientation, autonomic stability), with lower vagal
Nor did they use neuro-imaging techniques, such as electro-
tone, and with a greater relative right frontal EEG
encephalogram, event related potentials, and (functional)
activation. Elevated cortisol and norepinephrine, and
magnetic resonance imaging, or neuroendocrine measures.
lower dopamine and serotonin levels in the newborn were
In some studies of infants, the Bayley Scales of Infant
also found A structural equation model indicated
Development were used. Although these instruments are
that the less than optimal neonatal behavioural profile, in
useful as descriptive instruments and allow identification of
which 8–21% of the variance was explained, was related to
certain sensorimotor deficits, they are rather global
antenatal maternal depression and to cortisol and
measures. In addition, scores on these tests have proved to
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
be largely unrelated to scores on intelligence tests in later
hypothesis is that maternal stress hormones, and in
childhood (p. 33). Marker tasks provide more specific
particular, glucocorticoids, are transmitted across the
outcome measures. They are used in developmental
placenta . A second possible mechanism is via an effect
cognitive neurosciences and behavioural teratology
on uterine artery blood flow .
research to indirectly identify which underlyingstructure–function relations are altered. Neuro-imaging
4.1. Transfer of hormones across the placenta
techniques could elucidate some of the altered structure–function relations and underlying mechanism in a more
In utero exposure to abnormally high levels of maternal
direct manner. Using neuroendocrine measures, especially
glucocorticoids is one plausible mechanism by which
under stress-inducing situations, has the potential to
maternal stress may affect the fetus. However, the placenta
elucidate if and how the stress-regulating system is involved
is an effective barrier between the maternal and fetal
in the regulation problems of the offspring.
hormonal environments in humans, being rich in protective
A second weakness is that it is not always clear whether
enzymes such as monoamine oxidase A, peptidases, and
or not women were excluded who took medication such as
11b-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2, which converts
antidepressants during pregnancy
cortisol to inactive products such as cortisone The
Third, although maternal coping mechanisms and
impact of maternal stress on this enzyme is not known; there
characteristics such as optimism can interact
is some evidence that it is reduced in intrauterine growth
with anxiety/stress or have an independent effect, only a
restricted pregnancies .
few of the studies have included these measures. For
The links between maternal and fetal hormonal levels
instance, an unpublished result of study no. 6 revealed that
have been examined by studying the correlation between
use of emotion-focused coping (i.e. subscales expression of
maternal and fetal plasma levels for a range of hormones
emotions and social support of the Utrecht Coping list
(). Comparing levels of cortisol in paired maternal
had a positive effect on both psychomotor develop-
and fetal plasma samples, showed that fetal concentrations
ment (BZ6.13, p!0.0001) and mental development
were linearly related to maternal concentrations .
(BZ2.76, pZ0.044) and uniquely explained 17.8 and
As maternal concentrations are substantially higher than
6.5% of the variance, respectively, after control for the
fetal (over 10-fold), this is compatible with substantial
confounders listed under study no. 6 (State
(80–90%) metabolism of maternal cortisol during passage
anxiety was unrelated to this coping style (r [70]Z0.030;
across the placenta, and is in accord with in vivo and
ex vivo studies . However, it does suggest that if the
A fourth concern is that most of the studies have not
mother is stressed in a way that increases her own cortisol
looked for gender effects. Those studies that did (nos. 11,
level, this will be reflected in the hormonal milieu of the
12, 14–17) found some suggestion that boys were more
fetus. This mechanism cannot underlie the immediate links
susceptible to the influence of maternal anxiety and stress.
that have been observed between changes in maternal mood,
To conclude, the evidence for a link between antenatal
e.g. in anxiety while doing a cognitive test, and fetal
maternal anxiety/stress and regulation problems at the
behaviour as plasma cortisol takes about 10 min
cognitive, behavioural, and emotional levels in the child is
to respond to a stressor.
persuasive because this link has been replicated in 14
With both b-endorphin and noradrenaline
independent studies, with children ranging from birth up to
there was no significant correlation between maternal and
15-years-old. Moreover, this link generally persisted after
fetal plasma levels. Neither b-endorphin nor noradrenaline
controlling for post-natal maternal mood and/or other
is lipophilic, and neither would be expected to cross cell
potentially important pre- and post-natal confounders. The
membranes as readily as the steroid, cortisol. Corticotrophin
study of the timing, intensity and chronicity of anxiety/
releasing hormone (CRH) is correlated in the maternal and
stress, of maternal coping mechanisms and gender of the
fetal compartments of the placenta but to a lesser
child on a variety of neurodevelopmental aspects (includinghandedness) needs more attention. The use of marker tasks
of specific cognitive functions, neuro-imaging techniques,
Correlations between maternal and fetal hormone levels
and neuroendocrine measures could elucidate some of the
altered structure–function relationships and some under-
lying mechanisms.
4. Two physiological mechanisms by which the maternal
affective state may affect the fetus in humans
Giannakoulo-poulos 1999
Two mechanisms of transmission of anxiety/stress from
mother to fetus in humans have been suggested. One
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
degree than cortisol. Being a peptide, it is unlikely to cross
of 40 and more) had significantly worse uterine flow
from mother to fetus, and it is therefore more probable that
velocity waveform patterns than those in the lower anxiety
CRH is secreted into both compartments from the placenta,
groups. This finding on abnormal uterine blood flow
under some partial form of joint control. Testosterone, a
parameters in highly anxious women was recently con-
steroid like cortisol, is highly correlated in the two
firmed in a larger cohort where an association between
compartments, and it is plausible that there is some direct
maternal anxiety and uterine blood flow was present at 30
transfer from mother to fetus. Recently, it has also been
but not at 20 weeks of gestation (Jackson, Fisk and Glover;
shown that, unlike the norm in the adult, there is a positive
correlation between fetal plasma cortisol and testosterone
A study by Sjo¨stro¨m and colleagues aimed at
levels Cortisol and testosterone in the fetus are clearly
determining whether fetal circulation was affected by
not under identical control; there are likely to be several
maternal anxiety, found that, in the third trimester, fetuses
different determinants of fetal testosterone levels. Fetal
of women with high trait anxiety scores had higher indices
testosterone levels are higher in males than females but
of blood flow in the umbilical artery, and lower values in the
there is no difference in cortisol in the two sexes. Whereas
fetal middle cerebral artery, suggesting a change in blood
there is an increase in testosterone with gestational age in
distribution in favour of brain circulation in the fetus. These
females there is no such increase in cortisol over this age
results indicate that raised maternal anxiety, even within a
range. However, the mechanism of inter-related control of
normal population, had an influence on fetal cerebral
the HPA axis and testosterone production is different in the
fetus compared with the adult. Thus it may be that in the
We do not know whether these associations between
fetus some of the factors that cause raised fetal cortisol level
anxiety and Doppler patterns are acute or chronic. Further
may also cause an increase in testosterone level. This is
work is needed to determine whether overall anxiety during
compatible with a mechanism by which maternal stress may
pregnancy or even prior to or at conception, might affect
influence fetal development in ways associated with a more
later uterine artery blood flow patterns, or instead, whether
masculine profile, including an increase in mixed handed-
the association is only with the current emotional state. We
ness, ADHD and learning disabilities.
also need to determine whether the magnitude of the link
There have been very few studies examining the
between maternal anxiety and uterine blood flow is
function of the maternal HPA-axis during pregnancy in
sufficient to be of clinical significance.
relation to her emotional state. Obel observed that
In pregnant sheep infusion of noradrenaline decreased
evening, but not morning salivary cortisol was raised in
uterine blood flow, indicating the possibility that high
women with high perceived life stress at 30 weeks, but not
anxiety can cause acute changes in uterine artery blood flow
at 16 weeks of gestation. Rieger et al. found no
In addition, in sheep, reproductive tissues including
significant influence of perceived maternal stress on
the uterus are more sensitive to the vasoconstrictive effects
awakening cortisol response, neither in the first, nor in
of noradrenaline than other body tissues. However, other
the third trimester. Cortisol rises markedly at the end of
animal studies have also indicated the possibility that
gestation, and the mother's HPA-axis becomes desensi-
maternal stress or anxiety, early in gestation, might affect
tized to stressors as her pregnancy develops
the later uterine blood flow. In a rat model study cold stress
presumably due to the large amounts of CRH which are
early in pregnancy decreased trophoblastic invasion. This
released from the placenta. We do not know exactly when,
was followed by increased blood pressure, raised blood
and by how much this desensitization occurs.
catecholamine levels, and proteinuria in later pregnancyThe authors suggest they have produced a model for
4.2. Impaired uterine blood flow
preeclampsia, mediated by increased catecholamines caus-ing decreased trophoblastic invasion.
The hypothesis that anxiety in pregnant women is
To conclude, there is good evidence for a strong
associated with abnormal blood flow in the uterine arteries
correlation between maternal and fetal cortisol levels.
was tested using colour Doppler ultrasound to measure the
Thus if the mother is stressed in such a way as to raise
blood flow pattern and an according to standard procedures
fetal cortisol, the fetal environment may be changed in a
calculated Resistance Index (RI) A high RI indicates
way that could have long term effects. However, this
a greater resistance to blood flow, and is known to be
mechanism cannot underlie the immediate links between
associated with adverse obstetric outcome, particularly
maternal mood and fetal behaviour. Noradrenaline, which
intrauterine growth restriction and preeclampsia. The
can respond in seconds, does not appear to cross from
resulting lack of oxygen may also cause a direct stress to
mother to fetus, but may have an indirect effect via
the fetus. Significant associations between the RI in the
changes in the maternal muscular or vascular tone. This in
uterine artery and both state and trait anxiety were found in a
turn may cause stress to the fetus and raise cortisol levels.
sample of hundred women with singleton pregnancies,
However, much remains to be understood. We need to
measured between 28 and 32 weeks of gestation. Women in
know more about the biochemical correlates of normal
the highest anxiety groups (Spielberger's state anxiety score
variations and of high anxiety, stress and the response to
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
life events in the pregnant woman at different periods of
period. In lower parts of the brain (e.g. in the nuclei of the
gestation. We also need to know what happens when
brainstem and reticular formation) the first neurons are
cortisol levels are raised in the fetus. How does this affect
produced in the 4th week after conception (6th week
the development of the nervous system and of other
postmenstrual age). The basal ganglia become visible
systems, infant growth, age at delivery, and later
during the 6th postconceptional week, when the ganglionic
behaviour? We need to be aware that these may all be
eminence develops In the cerebral cortex, almost all
affected by different mechanisms.
neurons are generated at 6–18 weeks after conception. Aftertheir birth, neurons start migrating; the last born neuronsarrive at their final place in the cortex at about 23–24 weeks
5. Stress hormones and the developing fetal nervous
of gestation During migration, differen-
system: how are they related to behavioural/emotional
tiation of the neuron starts, resulting in the final phenotype
regulation problems in infants and children?
of the neuron. The prefrontal cortex differentiates ratherlate: only at 26–34 weeks of gestation is its basic 6-layered
There is evidence that complex functions such as
cytoarchitectonic pattern established . In contrast, in
behavioural and emotional regulation, are mediated through
the limbic system (e.g., the hippocampus, amygdala) and
the prefrontal cortex (PFC). The PFC has many subdivisions
limbic regions of the cortex (e.g. anterior cingulate cortex)
and collectively these areas have extensive and reciprocal
the major nuclei are already formed during the third and
connections with all sensory systems, cortical and sub-
fourth month; at 16 weeks the hippocampal area begins to
cortical motor system structures, subcortical arousal and
differentiate into the hippocampus proper and the dentate
attention functions, and with limbic and midbrain structures
gyrus Although differentiated early, the dentate gyrus
involved in affect, memory, and reward Behavioural
displays continued post-natal proliferation of granule cells;
functions are not localized in the PFC, rather the PFC
about 85% is formed at birth Proliferation of granule
(through the action of its subdivisions) seems to be essential
cells continues also in the cerebellum for several months
for the control of organized, integrated functioning
after birth .
For example, the medial part, including the anterior
Synaptic maturation includes the growth of axons and
cingulate cortex (ACC), controls a range of functions,
dendrites, axonal projections, synaptogenesis and myelina-
such as motivation, drive to perform, response selection,
tion. Correct timing and exclusion of inappropriate connec-
working memory, and novelty detection It is
tions (‘synaptic pruning') are essential for the maturation of
therefore of interest to determine how prenatal stress may
synaptic connections. Also apoptosis, or programmed cell
affect the development of the PFC and ACC and of areas
death, is necessary for proper development of the central
related to these regions.
nervous system, as about 50% of all generated neurons die.
Proper timing and guidance of neurogenesis, neuronal
In the neocortex, the first synapses are formed around 8
differentiation and migration, apoptosis, synaptogenesis and
weeks of gestation, although at a very low density .
myelination, are critical for the appropriate organization and
Different genes and their products (e.g. various transcription
functioning of the neocortex. These processes are controlled
factors and growth factors) are involved in early axon
by mechanisms intrinsic to the cell and processes extrinsic
guidance Until 23–24 weeks of gestation
to the cell, i.e. by genes and their products, by cell–cell
intrinsic (experience-independent) processes guide axonal
interactions, by interactions of cells with early neurotrans-
growth and synaptogenesis; at 23–24 weeks thalomo-
mitters and neuromodulators acting as growth factors
cortical circuits become functional and from then onwards
It is important to note that, although before 23 weeks of
(and throughout life) experience-dependent processes are
gestation these developmental processes are not driven by
important, first in expanding and afterwards in fine-tuning
activity that is modulated by sensory input, they never-
the neuronal circuits. Experience also induces modifications
theless can be altered This happens when environ-
in glial cells and cerebrovasculature . Clusters of
mental factors (e.g. viruses, tobacco, cocaine, cortisol)
genes are exclusively expressed in correlation with high
modulate the influence of intracellular and extracellular
levels of developmental plasticity (e.g. in the visual cortex
developmental signals. In general, the earlier the disturb-
this again illustrates the importance of the
ance occurs, the greater its potential influence on sub-
interaction between genes and environment (in casu
sequently occurring events and maturation, and finally, on
experience) for developmental cortical plasticity
the mature structure–function relationship .
In animal models, glucocorticoids are known to be
Although region-by-region differences in timing exist,
involved in fetal programming of the HPA-axis and
neurogenesis, neuronal differentiation and migration occur
neurotransmitter systems (for a review see ,
before the 7th month of gestation for most parts of the
and Owen et al. Antenatal maternal treatment with
nervous system. Knowledge of these differences is import-
synthetic glucocorticoids, such as betamethasone and
ant for delineating which cortical layers or areas (and hence
dexamethasone, has been shown to have a range of long-
processes) might have been altered by a disturbing
term effects on child behaviour and cognitive development
environmental agent, acting during a particular gestational
However, we currently know very little of
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
the influence of stress hormones on the developing human
consequences. Prenatal programming of the HPA-axis and
fetal nervous system. It is clear that, although cortisol is
of structure–function relationships controlled by the pre-
essential for normal brain development, exposure to
frontal cortex may contribute to regulation problems at the
excessive amounts has long-lasting effects on neuroendo-
cognitive, behavioural, and emotional level of children of
crine functioning and on behaviour. Glucocorticoids
mothers with high anxiety/stress during pregnancy. The
(cortisol in humans) are known to have profound effects
disturbance of the particular developmental processes
upon the developing brain and spinal cord; they can
taking place in specific brain layers and areas at the time
modulate cell proliferation and differentiation and synaptic
of antenatal maternal stress hormone release, in interaction
development in various brain regions If for
with the genetic susceptibility of the offspring and mediated
instance, in the third or fourth month of gestation, a
by later pre and post-natal environmental factors, will
teratogen such as cortisol modulates the influence of
determine the way in which cognitive, motor, arousal, and
developmental signals and disrupts neuronal migration,
emotional structure–function relationships are altered
this may result in abnormal cell density and cell position in
The ways in which the PFC integrates these altered
the different layers of the anterior cingulate cortex. This
processes presumably underlie the kind of behavioural/
pattern, which has been reported in postmortem cases of
emotional regulation problems these children will even-
schizophrenia and bipolar disorders results in
alterations of different neurotransmitter systems in thecorticolimbic region . During the onset of differen-tiation (e.g. at about 16 weeks in the hippocampus and
6. General conclusions
between 26–34 weeks in the prefrontal cortex) disturbancesby teratogens can alter the timetable of the expression of
This review shows that there is good evidence for a
several neurotransmitters, neuropeptides (e.g. CRH), and
direct link between antenatal anxiety/stress and fetal
their receptors. This in turn can alter receptor sensitivity as
behaviour observed by ultrasound from 27 to 28 weeks
well as dendritic outgrowth and formation of synapses, and
postmenstrual age onwards. There is also accumulating
change the balance between excitatory and inhibitory brain
evidence that there are links between maternal mood
during pregnancy and the long-term behaviour of her child.
Two recent studies are of interest in the context of
The fact that maternal anxiety/stress during pregnancy is
perinatal programming. Roberts et al. have recently
linked with later behaviour, even after controlling for
examined the relationship between the striatal dopamine
effects of post-natal maternal mood and other relevant
system integrity and behaviour in 5-to 7-year-old rhesus
prenatal and post-natal confounders, does suggest that, as
monkeys born from mothers that were exposed to stress
in animal models, a programming effect on the fetal brain
during late pregnancy . They have previously shown
is taking place. It is clear that many different underlying
altered HPA-axis function and behaviour in such offspring.
mechanisms and systems are involved in perinatal
In their new study, subjects from prenatal stress conditions
programming. Based on the available evidence it seems
showed an increase in the ratio of striatal dopamine D2
plausible that fetal programming of the HPA-axis, limbic
receptors and DA synthesis compared to controls, in a way
system, and prefrontal cortex may contribute to the
which they conclude supports a hypothesis linking striatal
regulation problems found in children of mothers who
function to behavioural inhibitory control. Lou et al. found a
were highly anxious/stressed during pregnancy. Many
link between high dopamine D2/3 receptor availability
questions remain on exactly how fetal programming
(examined with positron emission tomography) and inhi-
works in humans, and in which specific ways the timing,
bition failure (expressed in increased reaction time and
kind, intensity, and duration of environmental disturbances
reaction time variability during a computerized attention
are related to altered neurobehavioural development. The
task) in 27 prematurely born adolescents with ADHD
mechanisms underlying either direct links or fetal pro-
Interestingly, high dopamine receptor availability was
gramming in humans are only just starting to be
predicted by low neonatal cerebral blood flow. This could
contribute to a persistent deficiency in dopaminergic
However, there is enough evidence now to warrant active
neurotransmission. Results of these studies are congruent
research into prevention, intervention, and support pro-
with results of Durston et al. in which event-related
grams to reduce stress or anxiety during pregnancy and their
functional magnetic resonance imaging indicated that
effects on child outcome. These programs could include
children with ADHD did not activate fronto-striatal regions
stress reduction instructions (e.g. and cognitive-
during go/no-go tasks in the same manner as control
behavioural treatments to reduce anxiety from early
children, but rather relied on a more diffuse network of
gestation on, or even before conception (e.g.
Research on underlying mechanisms, on the effect of the
To conclude, disturbance of the delicate balance of
timing, intensity and duration of anxiety/stress, and the
factors guiding the precisely timed neocortical neurogenesis
effect of gender, can be carried out in parallel, and actually
and synaptogenesis during gestation can have long-term
would be helped by successful intervention strategies.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
It would also be of interest to use physiologically based
[17] Huizink AC, Mulder EJH, Buitelaar JK. Prenatal stress and risk for
measures of anxiety/stress and coping mechanisms during
psychopathology: specific effects or induction of general suscepti-bility? Psychol Bull 2004;130:115–42.
different gestational periods, and of regulation problems in
[18] Kofman O. The role of prenatal stress in the etiology of
the children after birth. The use of neuro-imaging
developmental behavioural disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
techniques and of different marker tasks for cognitive
development that can be reliably used from 7 to 8 m after
[19] Mulder EJH, Robles de Medina PG, Huizink AC, Van den
birth , would enable one to link the prenatal stress
Bergh BRH, Buitelaar JK, Visser GHA. Prenatal maternal stress:effects on pregnancy and the (unborn) child. Early Hum Dev 2002;
research in humans with behavioural teratology research
and cognitive developmental neuroscience.
[20] Wadhwa PD, Glynn L, Hobel CJ, Garite TJ, Porto M, Chicz-
There is evidence that up to 22% of the variance in
DeMet A, Wiglesworth AK, Sandman CA. Behavioral perinatology:
several behavioural problems is linked with prenatal
biobehavioral processes in human fetal development. Regul Pept
anxiety, stress, or depression. Mothers in the top 15% for
[21] Mozurkewich EL, Luke B, Avni M, Wolf FM. Working conditions
symptoms of antenatal anxiety have a doubled risk for
and adverse pregnancy outcome: a meta-analysis. Obstetr Gynecol
ADHD in their child at age 7. It is better to prevent these
developmental problems from arising than trying to treat
[22] Poland M, Giblin P, Lucas C, Sokol R. Psychobiological
them once established. A program to reduce maternal stress
determinants of pregnancy-induced hypertension. J Psychosomat
or anxiety in pregnancy may help.
Obstetr Gynaecol 1986;5:85–92.
[23] Buchbinder A, Sibai BM, Caritis S, MacPherson C, Hauth J,
Lindheimer MD, Klebanoff M, VanDorsten P, Landon M, Paul R.
Adverse perinatal outcomes are significantly higher in severegestational hypertension than in mild preeclampsia. Am J Obstet
[24] Hansen D, Lou HC, Olsen J. Serious life events and congenital
[1] Joffe JM. Prenatal determinants of behaviour. Oxford: Pergamon;
malformations: a national study with complete follow-up. Lancet
[2] Ferreira A. The pregnant woman's emotional attitude and its
[25] Van den Bergh BRH. Maternal emotions during pregnancy and fetal
reflection on the newborn. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1960;30:553–61.
and neonatal behavior. In: Nijhuis JG, editor. Fetal behaviour:
[3] Montagu A. Prenatal influences. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas;
Developmental and perinatal aspects. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univer-
sity Press; 1992. p. 157–78.
[4] Sontag L. The significance of fetal environmental differences. Am
[26] Visser GHA, Mulder EJH, Prechtl HFR. Studies on developmental
J Obstet Gynecol 1941;42:996–1003.
neurology in the human fetus. Dev Pharmacol Therap 1992;18:
[5] Gilbert S. The genome in its ecological context. Philosophical
perspectives on interspecies epigenesis. Ann NY Acad Sci 2002;981:
[27] de Vries JIP, Visser GHA, Prechtl HFR. The emergence of fetal
behaviour. I. Qualitative aspects. Early Hum Dev 1982;7:301–22.
[6] Gottlieb G. Synthesizing nature–nurture. Prenatal roots of instinctive
[28] Mulder EJH, Visser GHA, Bekedam DJ, Prechtl HFR. Emergence of
behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1997.
behavioural states in fetuses of type-1 diabetic women. Early Hum
[7] Barker D. The fetal origins of adult disease. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol
[29] Peirano P, Algarin C, Uauy R. Sleep–wake states and their
[8] Phillips DIW. Endocrine programming and fetal origins of adult
regulatory mechanisms throughout early human development.
disease. Trends Endocrinol Metab 2002;13:363.
J Pediatr 2003;143:70–9.
[9] Barker DJP. Fetal programming of coronary heart disease. Trends
[30] DiPietro JA, Hodgson DM, Costigan KA, Hilton SC, Johnson TRB.
Endocrinol Metab 2002;13:364–8.
Development of fetal movement-fetal heart rate coupling from 20
[10] Das UN. A perinatal strategy to prevent coronary heart disease.
weeks through term. Early Hum Dev 1996;44:139–51.
[31] DiPietro JA, Hodgson DM, Costigan KA, Hilton SC, Johnson TRB.
[11] Matthews SG. Early programming of the hypothalamo-pituitary-
Fetal neurobehavioral development. Child Dev 1996;67:2553–67.
adrenal axis. Trends Endocrinol Metab 2002;13:373–80.
[32] Herlenius E, Lagercrantz H. Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators
[12] Maccari S, Darnaudery M, Morley-Fletcher S, Zuena AR, Cinque C,
during early human development. Early Hum Dev 2001;65:21–37.
Van Reeth O. Prenatal stress and long-term consequences:
[33] Pace-Schott E, Hobson J. The neurobiology of sleep: genetics,
implications of glucocorticoid hormones. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nat Rev Neurosci
[13] Roberts AD, Moore CF, DeJesus OT, Barnhart TE, Larson JA,
[34] Hobson J, Pace-Schott E. The cognitive neuroscience of sleep:
Mukherjee J, Nickles RJ, Schueller MJ, Shelton SE, Schneider ML.
neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nat Rev Neurosci
Prenatal stress, moderate fetal alcohol, and dopamine system
function in rhesus monkeys. Neurotoxicol Teratol 2004;26:169–78.
[35] Saper CB, Chou TC, Scammell TE. The sleep switch: hypothalamic
[14] Schneider M, Roughton E, Koehler A, Lubach G. Growth and
control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci 2001;24:726–31.
development following prenatal stress exposure in primates: an
[36] Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. STAI Manual for the State-
examination of ontogenetic vulnerability. Child Dev 1999;70:263–74.
Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
[15] Weinstock M. Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain
Press; 1970.
morphology and behaviour of the offspring. Prog Neurobiol 2001;65:
[37] Ianniruberto A, Tajani E. Ultrasonographic study of fetal move-
ments. Sem Perinatol 1981;5:175–81.
[16] Schneider ML, Moore CF, Kraemer GW, Roberts AD, DeJesus OT.
[38] Van den Bergh BRH, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA, Poelmann-
The impact of prenatal stress, fetal alcohol exposure, or both on
Weesjes G, Bekedam DJ, Prechtl HFR. The effect of (induced)
development: perspectives from a primate model. Psychoneuroen-
maternal emotions on fetal behaviour: a controlled study. Early Hum
Dev 1989;19:9–19.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
[39] Van den Bergh BRH. The influence of maternal emotions during
[59] Ponirakis A, Susamn E, Stifter C. Negative emotionality and cortisol
pregnancy on fetal and neonatal behavior. Pre- and Perinatal
during adolescent pregnancy and its effects on infant health and
Psychology Journal 1990;5:119–30.
autonomic nervous system reactivity. Dev Psychobiol 1998;33:
[40] Groome L, Swiber M, Bentz L, Holland S, Atterbury J. Maternal
anxiety during pregnancy: effect on fetal behavior at 38 to 40 weeks
[60] Field T, Sandberg D, Garcia R, Vega-Lahr N, Goldstein S, Guy L.
of gestation. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1995;16:391–6.
Pregnancy problems, postpartum depression and early mother-infant
[41] DiPietro JA, Hilton SC, Hawkins M, Costigan KA, Pressman EK.
interactions. Dev Psychol 1985;21:1152–6.
Effects of socioeconomic status and psychosocial stress on the
[61] Davis EP, Snidman N, Wadhwa PD, Dunkel Schetter C, Glynn L,
development of the fetus. Ann NY Acad Sci 1999;896:356–8.
Sandman CA. Prenatal maternal anxiety and depression predict
[42] DiPietro JA, Hilton SC, Hawkins M, Costigan KA, Pressman EK.
negative behavioral reactivity in infancy. Infancy 2004;6(3) [in
Maternal stress and affect influence fetal neurobehavioral develop-
ment. Dev Psychol 2002;38:659–68.
[62] Vaughn B, Bradley C, Joffe L, Seifer R, Barglow P. Maternal
characteristics measured prenatally are predictive of ratings of
[43] Sjo¨stro¨m K, Valentin L, Thelin T, Mars¸a´l K. Maternal anxiety in late
temperamental difficulty on the Carey infant temperament ques-
pregnancy: effect on fetal movements and fetal heart rate. Early Hum
tionnaire. Dev Psychol 1987;23:152–61.
[63] Huizink AC. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictor
[44] Bartha J, Martinez-del-Fresno P, Romero-Carmona R, Hunter A,
of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;
Comino-Delgado R. Maternal anxiety and fetal behavior at 15 weeks
gestation. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 2003;22:57–62.
[64] Huizink AC, Robles de Medina PG, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA,
[45] Mulder EJH, Robles de Medina PG, Huizink AC, Buitelaar JK,
Buitelaar JK. Stress during pregnancy is associated with develop-
Visser GHA. Prenatal maternal stress: effects on fetal functional
mental outcome in infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003;44:
development. Society for research in child development conference.
April 24–27, 2003; Tampa, Florida, USA.
[65] Brouwers EPM, Van Baar AL, Pop VJM. Maternal anxiety during
Niederhofer H, Reiter A. Prenatal maternal stress, prenatal fetal
pregnancy and subsequent infant development. Infant Behavior Dev
movements and perinatal temperament factors influence behavior
and school marks at the age of 6 years. Fetal Diagn Ther 2004;19:
[66] Laplante DP, Barr RG, Brunet A, Galbaud du Fort G, Meaney ML,
Saucier J-P, Zelazo PR, King S. Stress during pregnancy affects
[47] Paine L, Strobino D, Witter F, Johnson T. Population differences
general intellectual and language functioning in human toddlers.
affect nonstress test reactivity. J Perinatol 1991;11:41–5.
Pediatric Res 2004;56:400–10.
[48] DiPietro JA, Costigan KA, Gurewitsch ED. Fetal response to
[67] O'Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Beveridge M, Glover V. Maternal
induced maternal stress. Early Hum Dev 2003;74:125–38.
antenatal anxiety and children's behavioural/emotional problems at
[49] Monk C, Fifer W, Sloan R, Myers M, Trien L, Hurtado A. Maternal
4 years: report from the avon longitudinal study of parents and
stress responses and anxiety during pregnancy: effects on fetal heart
children. Br J Psychiatry 2002;180:502–8.
rate. Dev Psychobiol 2000;36:67–77.
[68] O'Connor TG, Heron J, Glover V. Antenatal anxiety predicts child
[50] Monk C, Myers MM, Sloan RP, Ellman LM, Fifer WP. Effects of
women's stress-elicited physiological activity and chronic anxiety
depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41:1470–7.
on fetal heart rate. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2003;24:32–8.
[69] Martin RP, Noyes JN, Wisenbaker J, Huttunen MO. Prediction of
[51] Sandman CA, Glynn L, Wadhwa PD, Chicz-DeMet A, Porto M,
early childhood negative emotionality and inhibition from maternal
Garite T. Maternal hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal disregulation
distress during pregnancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 1999;45:
during the third trimester influences human fetal responses. Dev
[70] O'Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V, the ALSPAC Study
[52] Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA, Garite TJ. The neurobiology of stress in
Team. Maternal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in
human pregnancy: implications for prematurity and development of
children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol
the fetal central nervous system. Prog Brain Res 2001;133:131–42.
[53] ten Hof J, Nijhuis IJM, Nijhuis JG, Narayan H, Taylor DJ,
[71] Rodriguez A, Bohlin G. Are maternal smoking and stress during
Visser GHA, Mulder EJH. Quantitative analysis of fetal general
pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children? J Child PsycholPsychiatry 2004 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00359.X.
movements: methodological considerations. Early Hum Dev 1999;
[72] Van den Bergh BRH, Marcoen A. High antenatal maternal anxiety is
related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems and anxiety in
[54] Mulder EJH, Robles de Medina PG, Beekhuijzen M, Wijnberger DE,
8/9-year-olds. Child Dev 2004;75:1085–97.
Visser GHA. Fetal stimulation and activity state. Lancet 2001;357:
[73] Van den Bergh BRH, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Stevens V, Stiers
P, Marcoen A, Lagae L. High antenatal maternal anxiety is related
[55] Carlson B, LaBarba R. Maternal emotionality during pregnancy and
to impulsivity during performance on cognitive tasks in 14- and
reproductive outcome. Int J Behav Dev 1979;2:342–6.
15-year-olds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, in press, doi: 10.1016/
[56] Van den Bergh BRH. de emotionele toestand Van de (zwangere)
vrouw, obstetrische complicaties en het gedrag en de ontwikkeling
[74] Obel C. Epidemiological studies of stress during pregnancy and fetal
Van de foetus en Van het kind tot de leeftijd Van zeven maanden
brain development. Doctorate Thesis. Denmark: Faculty of Health
[The emotional state of the (pregnant) woman, obstetrical compli-
Sciences, University of Aarhus; 2003.
cations and the behaviour and development of fetus and child until
[75] Van den Bergh BRH. Long-term effects of maternal anxiety during
seven months after birth]. Unpublished Doctorate Thesis. Catholic
pregnancy: behavioral and emotional problems, attention and
University Leuven, Belgium; 1989.
inhibitory control in 14- and 15-year-olds. Paper presented at
[57] Rieger M, Pirke K-M, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Wurmser H,
Colloquium of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and
Papousek M, Hellhammer D. Influence of stress during pregnancy
Sciences: prenatal programming of behavior, physiology and
on neonatal behavior. Ann NY Acad Sci 2004;1032:1–3.
cognition. February 17–19, 2004; Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
[58] Lou H, Hansen D, Nordentoft M, Pryds O, Jensen F, Nim J,
[76] Rutter M, Silberg J, O'Connor T, Simonoff E. Genetics and child
Hemmingsen R. Prenatal stressors of human life affect fetal brain
psychiatry. I. Advances in quantitative and molecular genetics.
development. Dev Med Child Neurol 1994;36:826–32.
J Child Psychol Psychiatr 1999;40:3–18.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
[77] Roubertoux P, Nosten-Bertrand M, Carlier M. Additive and
[95] Lobel M, DeVincent CJ, Kaminer A, Meyer BA. The impact of
interactive effects of genotype and maternal environment. Adv
prenatal maternal stress and optimistic disposition on birth outcomes
Study Behav 1990;19:205–47.
in medically high-risk women. Health Psychol 2000;19:544–53.
[78] Gunnar M. Early adversity and the development of stress reactivity
[96] Yali A, Lobel M. Stress-resistance resources and coping in
and regulation. In: Nelson C, editor. The effects of early adversity on
pregnancy. Anxiety, Stress Coping 2002;15:289–309.
neurobehavioral development. The Minnesota symposia on child
[97] Huizink AC, Robles de Medina PG, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA,
psychology, vol. 31. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2000. p. 163–200.
Buitelaar JK. Coping in normal pregnancy. Ann Behav Med 2002;
[79] Linnet KM, Dalsgaard S, Obel C, Wisborg K, Henriksen TB,
Rodriguez A, Kotimaa A, Moilanen I, Thomsen PH, Olsen J,
[98] Gitau R, Fisk N, Teixeira J, Cameron A, Glover V. Fetal
Jarvelin M-R. Maternal lifestyle factors in pregnancy risk of
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal stress responses to invasive pro-
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated behaviors:
cedures are independent of maternal responses. J Clin Endocrinol
review of the current evidence. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:1028–40.
[80] Heron J, O'Connor TG, Evans J, Golding J, Glover V. The course of
[99] Sjo¨stro¨m K, Valentin L, Thelin T, Mars¸a´l K. Maternal anxiety in late
anxiety and depression through pregnancy and the postpartum in a
pregnancy and fetal hemodynamics. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod
community sample. J Affect Disord 2004;80:65–73.
[81] Huizink AC. Prenatal stress and its effect on infant development.
[100] Teixeira JMA, Fisk NM, Glover V. Association between maternal
Doctorate Thesis. The Netherlands: University of Utrecht; 2001.
anxiety in pregnancy and increased uterine artery resistance index:
[82] Van der Ploeg HM, Defares PB, Spielberger CD. Handleiding bij de
cohort based study. BMJ 1999;318:153–7.
zelf-beoordelings vragenlijst ZBV: Een nederlandstalige bewerking
[101] Challis JRG, Sloboda D, Matthews SG, Holloway A, Alfaidy N,
Van de Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI-DY.
Patel FA, Whittle W, Fraser M, Moss TJM, Newnham J. The fetal
[Manual of the self-evaluation questionnaire: a Dutch version of
placental hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, parturition
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets
and post-natal health. Mol Cell Endocrinol 2001;185:135–44.
and Zeitlinger B.V.; 1980.
[102] McTernan CL, Draper N, Nicholson H, Chalder SM, Driver P,
[83] Field T, Diego M, Dieter J, Hernandez-Reif M, Schanberg S,
Hewison M, Kilby MD, Stewart PM. Reduced placental 11{beta}-
Kuhn C, Yando R, Bendell D. Prenatal depression effects on the fetus
hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 mRNA levels in human
and the newborn. Infant Behav Dev 2004;27:216–29.
pregnancies complicated by intrauterine growth restriction: an
[84] Diego MA, Field T, Jones NA, Hernandez-Reif M, Cullen C,
analysis of possible mechanisms. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2001;
Schanberg S, Kuhn C. EEG responses to mock facial expressions by
infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behav Dev 2004;27:150–62.
[103] Gitau R, Cameron A, Fisk N, Glover V. Fetal exposure to maternal
[85] Zuckerman B, Bauchner H, Parker S, Cabral H. Maternal depressive
cortisol. Lancet 1998;352:707–8.
symptoms during newborn irritability. Dev Behav Pediatr 1990;11:
[104] Murphy B, Clark S, Donald I, Pinsky M, Vedady D. Conversion of
maternal cortisol to cortisone during placental transfer to the human
[86] Dawson G, Ashman SB, Hessl D, Spieker S, Frey K, Panagiotides H,
fetus. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1974;118:538–41.
Embry L. Autonomic and brain electrical activity in securely- and
[105] Benediktsson R, Calder A, Edwards C, Seckl J. Placental 11 beta-
insecurely-attached infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behav Dev
hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase: a key regulator of fetal glucocorti-
coid exposure. Clin Endocrinol 1997;46:161–6.
[87] Dawson G, Ashman SB. On the origins of a vulnerability to
[106] Gitau R, Fisk N, Glover V. Human fetal and maternal corticotrophin
depression: the influence of the early social environment on the
releasing hormone responses to acute stress. Arch Dis Child Fetal
development of psychobiological systems related to risk for affective
Neonatal Ed 2004;89:F29–F32.
disorder. In: Nelson C, editor. The effects of early adversity on
[107] Giannakoulopoulos X, Teixeira J, Fisk N, Glover V. Human fetal
neurobehavioral development. The Minnesota symposia on child
and maternal noradrenaline responses to invasive procedures. Pediatr
psychology, vol. 31, 2000. p. 245–80.
[88] Ma¨ki P, Veijola J, Rasanen P, Joukamaa M, Valonen P, Jokelainen J,
[108] Gitau R, Adams D, Fisk N, Glover V. Fetal plasma testosterone
Isohanni M. Criminality in the offspring of antenatally depressedmothers: a 33-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth
correlates positively with cortisol. Arch Dis Child 2004 [in press].
Cohort. J Affect Disord 2003;74:273–8.
[109] Schulte H, Weisner D, Allolio B. The corticotrophin releasing
[89] Obel C, Hedegaard M, Henriksen T, Secher N, Olsen J. Psycho-
hormone test in late pregnancy: lack of adrenocorticotrophin and
logical factors in pregnancy and mixed-handedness in the offspring.
cortisol response. Clin Endocrinol 1990;33:99–106.
Dev Med Child Neurol 2003;45:557–61.
[110] Kammerer M, Adams D, Castelberg BB, Glover V. Pregnant women
[90] Glover V, O'Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J. Antenatal maternal
become insensitive to cold stress. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2002;
anxiety is linked with atypical handedness in the child. Early Hum
[111] Rosenfeld C, West J. Circulatory response to systemic infusion of
[91] Rose S, Feldman J. The relation of very low birth weight to basic
norepinephrine in the pregnant ewe. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1977;127:
cognitive skills in infancy and childhood. In: Nelson C, editor. The
effects of early adversity on neurobehavioral development. The
[112] Kanayama N, Tsujimura R, She L, Maehara K, Terao T. Cold-
Minnesota symposia on child psychology, vol. 31, 2000. p. 31–60.
induced stress stimulates the sympathetic nervous system, causing
[92] Diamond A. A model system for studying the role of dopamine in the
hypertension and proteinuria in rats. J Hypertens 1997;15:383–9.
prefrontal cortex during early development in humans. In:
[113] Miller E, Cohen J. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex
Johnson M, Munakata Y, Gilmore R, editors., 2002. p. 217–30.
function. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24:167–202.
[93] Jacobson S, Jacobson J. Teratogenic insult and neurobehavioural
[114] Spreen OT, Risser AH, Edgell D. Developmental neuropsychology.
function in infancy and childhood. In: Nelson C, editor. The effects
New York: Oxford University Press; 1995.
of early adversity on neurobehavioral development. The Minnesota
[115] Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in
symposia on child psychology, vol. 31. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2000.
anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cognitive Sci 2000;4:215–22.
p. 61–112.
[116] Carter CS, Macdonald AM, Botvinick M, Ross LL, Stenger VA,
[94] McConnell PJ, Linn K, Filkins K. Depression and pregnancy: use of
Noll D, Cohen JD. Parsing executive processes: strategic vs.
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in pregnancy. Primary Care
evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl
Update OB/GYNS 1998;5:11–15.
Acad Sci USA 2000;97:1944–8.
B.R.H. Van den Bergh et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (2005) 237–258
[117] Paus T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control,
[138] Welberg LAM, Seckl JR, Holmes MC. Prenatal glucocorticoid
drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci 2001;2:417–24.
programming of brain corticosteroid receptors and corticotrophin-
[118] Nowakowski RS, Hayes NL. In: Johnson MH, Munakata Y,
releasing hormone: Possible implications for behaviour. Neuro-
Gilmore ROE, editors. General principles of CNS development,
2002. p. 57–82.
[139] Owen D, Andrews MH, Matthews SG. Maternal adversity,
[119] Bourgeois J. Synaptogenesis, heterochrony and epigenesis in the
glucocorticoids and programming of neuroendocrine function and
mammalian cortex. Acta Paediatr 1997;(Suppl 442):27–33.
[120] Levitt P, Reinoso B, Jones L. The critical impact of early cellular
environment on neuronal development. Prev Med 1998;27:180–3.
[140] Andrews MH, Matthews SG. Antenatal glucocorticoids: Is there
[121] Weaver ICG, la Plante P, Weaver S, Parent A, Sharma S, Diorio J,
cause for concern? Fetal Matern Med Rev 2003;14:329–54.
Chapman KE, Seckl JR, Szyf M, Meaney MJ. Early environmental
[141] French NP, Hagan R, Evans SF, Mullan A, Newnham JP. Repeated
regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression:
antenatal corticosteroids: effects on cerebral palsy and childhoodbehavior. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2004;190:588–95.
Characterization of intracellular mediators and potential genomic
[142] Meyer-Bahlburg HF, Dolezal C, Baker SW, Carlson AD, Obeid JS,
target sites. Mol Cell Endocrinol 2001;185:205–18.
New MI. Cognitive and motor development of children with and
[122] Rakic P. Pre- and post-developmental neurogenesis in primates. Clin
without congenital adrenal hyperplasia after early-prenatal dexa-
Neurosci Res 2002;2:29–39.
methasone. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2004;89:610–4.
[123] Rakic P. Intrinsic and extrinsic determinants of neocortical
[143] Coe CL, Kramer M, Czeh B, Gould E, Reeves AJ, Kirschbaum C,
parcellation: a radial unit model. In: Johnson M, Munakata Y,
Fuchs E. Prenatal stress diminishes neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus
editors. Brain development and cognition. A reader. 2nd ed. Malden,
of juvenile Rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:1025–34.
MA: Black Well Publishers; 2002. p. 83–100.
[144] de Nicola AF, Ferrini M, Gonzalez SL, Deniselle MC, Grillo CA,
[124] Levitt P. Structural and functional maturation of the developing
Piroli G, Saravia F, de Kloet ER. Regulation of gene expression by
primate brain. J Pediatr 2003;143:35–45.
corticoid hormones in the brain and spinal cord. J Steroid Biochem
[125] Kostovic I, Judas M, Petanjek Z, Simic G. Ontogenesis of goal-
Mol Biol 1998;65:253–72.
[145] de Kloet ER, Reul JMHM, Sutanto W. Corticosteroids and the brain.
J Psychophysiol 1995;19:85–102.
J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 1990;37:387–94.
[126] Seress L, Abraham H, Tornoczky T, Kosztolanyi G. Cell formation
[146] Yu IT, Lee S-H, Lee Y-S, Son H. Differential effects of
in the human hippocampal formation from mid-gestation to the late
corticosterone and dexamethasone on hippocampal neurogenesis in
post-natal period. Neuroscience 2001;105:831–43.
vitro. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 2004;317:484–90.
[127] Abraham H, Tornoczky T, Kosztolanyi G, Seress L. Cell formation
[147] Benes FM, Vincent SL, Todtenkopf M. The density of pyramidal and
in the cortical layers of the developing human cerebellum. Int J Dev
nonpyramidal neurons in anterior cingulate cortex of schizophrenic
and bipolar subjects. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:395–406.
[128] Eagleson KL, Pimenta AF, Burns MM, Fairfull LD, Cornuet PK,
[148] Benes FM. Emerging principles of altered neural circuitry in
Zhang L, Levitt P. Distinct domains of the limbic system-associated
schizophrenia. Brain Res Rev 2000;31:251–69.
membrane protein (LAMP) mediate discrete effects on neurite
[149] Ladd C, Huot R, Thrivikraman K, Nemeroff C, Meaney M,
outgrowth. Mol Cell Neurosci 2003;24:725–40.
Plotsky P. Long-term behavioral and neuroendocrine adaptations
[129] Frappe´ I, Gaillard A, Roger M. Attraction exerted in vivo by grafts of
to adverse and early experiences. Prog Brain Res 2000;122:81–103.
embryonic neocortex on developing thalamic axons. Exp Neurol
[150] Schneider M, Clarke A, Kraemer G, Rougthon E, Lubach G, Rimm-
Kauffman S, Schmidt D, Elbert M. Prenatal stress alters brain biogenic
[130] Zhang J, Jin Z, Bao Z. Disruption of gradient expression of Zic3
amine levels in primates. Dev Psychopathol 1998;10:427–40.
resulted in abnormal intraretinal axon projection. Development
[151] Lou H, Rosa P, Pryds O, Karrebaek H, Lunding J, Cumming P,
Gjedde A. ADHD: Increased dopamine receptor availability linked
[131] Elman JF, Bates EA, Johnson MH, Karmiloff-Smith A, Parisi D,
to attention deficit and low neonatal cerebral blood flow. Dev MedChild Neurol 2004;46:179–83.
Plunkett K. Rethinking innateness: a connectionist perspective on
[152] Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti I-M,
development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998.
Yang Y, Ulug AM, Casey BJ. Differential patterns of striatal
[132] Nelson C, Bloom F. Child development and neuroscience. Child Dev
activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biol
[133] Greenough W, Black J, Wallace C. Experience and brain develop-
[153] Rice D, Barone S. Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing
ment. Child Dev 1987;58:533–8.
nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ
[134] Kingsbury M, Finlay B. The cortex in multidimensional space:
Health Perspect 2000;108:511–33.
where do cortical areas come from? Dev Sci 2001;4:125–57.
[154] Andersen SL. Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability
[135] O'Leary D. Do cortical areas emerge from a protocortex?. In:
or window of opportunity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2003;27:3–18.
Johnson M, Munakata Y, Gilmore R, editors. Brain development and
[155] Stanwood GD, Levitt P. Drug exposure early in life: functional
cognition. A reader. 2nd ed. Malden, MA: Black Well Publishers;
repercussions of changing neuropharmacology during sensitive
2002. p. 217–30.
periods of brain development. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2004;4:65–71.
[136] Prasad SS, Kojic LZ, Li P, Mitchell DE, Hachisuka A, Sawada J,
[156] Urizar Jr G, Milazzo M, Le H, Delucchi K, Sotelo R, Munoz R.
Gu Q, Cynader MS. Gene expression patterns during enhanced
Impact of stress reduction instructions on stress and cortisol levels
periods of visual cortex plasticity. Neuroscience 2002;111:35–45.
during pregnancy. Biol Psychol 2004;67:275–82.
[137] Grossman A, Churchill J, McKinney B, Kodish I, Otte S,
[157] Facchinetti F, Tarabusi M, Volpe A. Cognitive-behavioral treatment
Greenough W. Experience effects on brain development: Possible
decreases cardiovascular and neuroendocrine reaction to stress in
contributions to psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003;
women waiting for assisted reproduction. Psychoneuroendocrinol-
Source: http://www.waimh-vlaanderen.be/files/NeurBioRev2005_VandenberghetalRev.pdf
ACUPUNCTURE AND PRE-MENSTRUAL SYNDROME About pre-menstrual syndrome Most women of reproductive age experience at least mild premenstrual symptoms at some time in their lives (O'Brien 1987). However, around 2–10% of women have premenstrual symptoms that severely disrupt daily living (O'Brien 1987, DTB 1992, Wittchen 2002). These more troublesome symptoms are usually termed ‘premenstrual syndrome' (PMS), if they comprise recurrent psychological and/or physical symptoms that occur specifically during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle and usually resolve by the end of menstruation (O'Brien 1987).
Sustainable Purchasing Guidelines Toyota Motor Europe Version 2013 Table of contents A. Toyota Motor Europe – CSR Guiding Principles at Toyota Toyota CSR Policy: Contribution towards Sustainable Development Basic Policies at Toyota Purchasing 1. Fair competition based on an open-door policy 2. Mutual benefit based on mutual trust